Longitudinal Study before and after the Fukushima Disaster. Vivianne H. M. Visschers* Michael Siegrist. ETH Zurich

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Longitudinal Study before and after the Fukushima Disaster. Vivianne H. M. Visschers* Michael Siegrist. ETH Zurich"

Transcription

1 1 How a Nuclear Power Plant Accident Influences Acceptance of Nuclear Power: Results of a Longitudinal Study before and after the Fukushima Disaster Vivianne H. M. Visschers* Michael Siegrist ETH Zurich Intitute for Environmental Decisions (IED), Consumer Behavior Zurich, Switzerland * Address correspondence to Vivianne Visschers, ETH Zurich, Institute for Environmental Decisions, Consumer Behavior, Universitaetsstrasse 22 CHN J75.2, CH-8092 Zurich, Switzerland. Tel , Fax , vvisschers@ethz.ch.

2 2 Abstract Major nuclear accidents, such as the recent accident in Fukushima, Japan, have been shown to decrease the public s acceptance of nuclear power. However, little is known about how a serious accident affects people s acceptance of nuclear power and the determinants of acceptance. We conducted a longitudinal study (N = 790) in Switzerland: one survey was done five months before and one directly after the accident in Fukushima. We assessed acceptance, perceived risks, perceived benefits, and trust related to nuclear power stations. In our model, we assumed that both benefit and risk perceptions determine acceptance of nuclear power. We further hypothesized that trust influences benefit and risk perceptions and that trust before a disaster relates to trust after a disaster. Results showed that the acceptance and perceptions of nuclear power as well as its trust were more negative after the accident. In our model, perceived benefits and risks determined the acceptance of nuclear power stations both before and after Fukushima. Trust had strong effects on perceived benefits and risks, at both times. People s trust before Fukushima strongly influenced their trust after the accident. Additionally, perceived benefits before Fukushima correlated with perceived benefits after the accident. Thus, the nuclear accident did not seem to have changed the relations between the determinants of acceptance. Even after a severe accident, the public may still consider the benefits as relevant, and trust remains important for determining their risk and benefit perceptions. A discussion of the benefits of nuclear power seems most likely to affect the public s acceptance of nuclear power, even after a nuclear accident. Keywords: nuclear accident, Fukushima, longitudinal study, acceptance, social trust

3 3 1. INTRODUCTION On March 11, 2011, an enormous earthquake and its subsequent tsunami devastated the cooling systems of the Fukushima Daiichi nuclear power plant in Japan. As a result, vapor, cooling water, and explosive particles with radioactive elements were released or leaked from three reactors into the environment. The authorities decided to evacuate people living in a 30km radius around the power plant, and a 20km zone was declared a no-go area. The International Atomic Energy Association calculated the nuclear accident to be at level 7 on the International Nuclear and Radiological Event Scale (INES), this being the highest level. (1) Soon after the accident, discussions about the safety and future of nuclear power plants began in many countries around the world. Previous research shows that a major accident at a nuclear reactor strongly affects laypeople s attitudes toward nuclear power (e.g., refs. 2-4). The accident in Japan provided an opportunity to test additional hypotheses that have not been addressed in previous research regarding the effect of a nuclear accident on public perception. In a longitudinal study that included two waves, we examined how the accident at the nuclear power plant in Fukushima changed people s trust, their perceptions of benefits and risks, and their acceptance of nuclear power in the German-speaking part of Switzerland Public Perception of Nuclear Power after a Nuclear Accident After the accidents at the nuclear power plants in Chernobyl in 1986 and on Three Mile Island (TMI) in 1979, a large number of studies investigated people s attitudes toward nuclear power. Some studies compared laypeople s opinions of nuclear power before and after the disasters by comparing the findings of different samples both over time (5-7) and between countries. (8, 9) Additionally, a few longitudinal studies investigated attitudes assessed before and

4 4 after the catastrophic events, thus using the same sample of respondents. (2-4) These studies will be discussed in more detail below. Also, a few polls in the USA and Europe monitored the public s opinion of nuclear power (see refs for overviews). We are, of course, aware of the fact that a large number of studies have been conducted to investigate people s attitudes and acceptance of nuclear power stations and related facilities, as well as the determinants of these opinions (e.g., refs ) This introduction, however, concentrates on studies that intended to examine the effect of a nuclear accident on the public s opinion of this energy technology. Even though opinion polls revealed that the public s support for nuclear power had been decreasing since the mid-1970s in Europe and the US, (5, 10, 12) the two previous nuclear accidents (5, 7, seemed to have had additional negative impacts on people s attitudes toward nuclear power. 10, 11, 19-21) Hence, these nuclear accidents accelerated the decline in public support of nuclear power. Several studies investigated possible determinants of the more negative attitudes resulting from the nuclear accidents (e.g., refs. 8, 9, 20, 21). In countries that were exposed to higher radiological doses after Chernobyl, people s attitudes toward nuclear power became more negative than those of people in countries in which the exposure levels were lower. (9) Thus, the extent to which a nuclear accident challenged previous beliefs about nuclear power appeared to affect people s attitudes. A study in Sweden examining various specific groups (e.g., farmers, parents, and adolescents) showed that perceived risks of nuclear power were important predictors of people s nuclear power evaluation. (20) However, only negative consequences were included in this study; positive issues, such as perceived benefits of nuclear power, were not considered. A study conducted in the US after TMI included an assessment of the expected benefits of a new, local nuclear power plant and an assessment of its expected risks in order to explain the acceptance of

5 5 this new plant. (21) Additionally, respondents were asked to estimate the influence of the accident at TMI on their acceptance of the new plant. Both expected risks and expected benefits influenced acceptance of the local plant. The self-reported influence of TMI affected expected risks to a larger extent than did expected benefits. However, a self-reported measure of TMI influence is prone to biases, which may have indirectly resulted in perceived risks having a larger influence than perceived benefits. In a study that compared perceptions of nuclear power before and after Chernobyl between two different samples, indications of slightly higher risk perceptions were found after Chernobyl (though only among students) compared to before. (6) More interestingly, after the accident, people reported higher levels of dread, more knowledge about the risks of nuclear power, and a smaller degree of perceived severity of a nuclear accident. The latter was probably a result of increased knowledge about the number of casualties Previous Longitudinal Studies Regarding a Nuclear Accident To the best of our knowledge, only three studies have examined the impact of a nuclear accident on laypeople s perception of nuclear power while using a longitudinal, within-subjects design that is, including a survey before the accident and one survey or several surveys after the accident. (2-4) Following Menard, (22) we define a longitudinal study as a study that measures the same variables in the same respondents at two or more time points. All three previous longitudinal studies were conducted before and after the Chernobyl accident. Verplanken (3) was able to follow 154 Dutch people before and after Chernobyl (one month after and, again, six months after), measuring their attitudes and beliefs regarding nuclear power. A third follow-up survey was done nineteen months after the event, which included 103 respondents from the original sample. Although nuclear attitudes had become significantly more

6 6 negative directly after the accident compared to before the accident, they almost returned to pre- Chernobyl levels six months after the accident. However, nineteen months after the accident, public opinion again became more negative, perhaps due to widespread media coverage. Moreover, one-and-a-half years after the event, the accident appeared to increase respondents perceived probability of a nuclear accident occurring and to decrease their benefit perception of nuclear power. The correlations between nuclear attitudes and perceived risks and perceived benefits appeared strong at all times. However, the relation between people s nuclear power attitudes and their perceived probability of a serious nuclear accident occurring had become even stronger by the time the last measurement was completed, nineteen months after the accident. An increased risk perception may explain the increase in negative attitudes after one-and-a-half years compared to those attitudes held directly after the nuclear catastrophe. Nevertheless, Verplanken (3) did not examine the causal relation between risk perception and nuclear attitudes. The second longitudinal study regarding Chernobyl was conducted in England. (4) It consisted of pre- and post-incident measurements of public opinion taken around the time of the nuclear accident/. The measurements included 135 inhabitants of a town close to a nuclear power plant and residents who did not live far from an onshore oil installation. Acceptance of nuclear power stations was compared to that of oil wells, coal power stations, and other industrial plants. After Chernobyl, respondents showed less acceptance of nuclear power, whereas acceptance of the other plants had not changed. Moreover, Eiser and colleagues (4) related post-chernobyl attitudes toward nuclear power to acceptance, perceived risk from a hypothetical nuclear power station at various distances, respondents attention to news about the nuclear accident, and their level of fear evoked by this news. Prior negative nuclear attitudes were associated with a decrease in acceptance after the accident, more attention being paid to the news, and higher levels of fear

7 7 being felt about this news after the accident. Respondents risk perception was not influenced by their distance to the hypothetical nuclear plant. Another longitudinal study was done in the northwest of the USA in a town near a nuclear power plant. (2) The area was affected by a plume from Chernobyl that contained small amounts of radioactive material. The study included a survey five months before Chernobyl and a second survey one month after the accident. After the accident, the 69 respondents perceived fewer personal health effects, such as cancer, from the local nuclear power plant, although they were more occupied with the topic of nuclear power than they had been before the accident. Neither their perceived likelihood nor their perceived dread of a nuclear accident had changed since the incident. Midden and Verplanken (23) observed that attitudes toward nuclear power and beliefs about the consequences of nuclear power were not stable between several measurement points during the two years after the accident at Chernobyl. However, the authors could not compare these beliefs and attitudes to those before the accident. In sum, only a few studies investigated the effects of a nuclear accident using a longitudinal design. The two European full-longitudinal studies noted above found that people s views of nuclear power stations became more negative after the Chernobyl catastrophe. (3, 4) However, the attitude changes were relatively small. All three full-longitudinal studies could only compare acceptance of nuclear power between the two measures. (2-4) The possible determinants of acceptance, such as attitudes and beliefs, were only assessed after the catastrophe. Moreover, the studies had small sample sizes, ranging from less than 70 to 150 respondents Current Situation Since the accidents in Chernobyl and on TMI, a few developments have taken place in the

8 8 areas of science, society, politics, and the environment as part of a nuclear renaissance, i.e., a revival of nuclear energy production. (24) First, the political climate has changed. In the 1980s, during the Cold War, nuclear power came to be associated with atomic weapons and nuclear war. (10) People s fears about the latter may have affected their opinions of nuclear power stations. A study conducted in 2009 showed that Swiss people mainly associated nuclear power with energy. (25) In comparison, its associations with risks, accidents, and military use were much less frequently reported. Second, the need to reduce CO 2 emissions to mitigate climate change contributed to a more positive perception of nuclear power. (26) The influence of nuclear power s climate benefits on laypeople s acceptance appeared significant but relatively low. (27) A third, more important factor that seemed to drive the nuclear renaissance was the perceived economic benefits of nuclear power (e.g., ref. 27) A country with nuclear power plants can be less dependent on other countries, has a secure energy supply, and produces relatively cheap and sufficient energy to meet the needs of increasing energy consumption. Moreover, the total costs (including the environmental burden) of nuclear power appear to be much lower than those of fossil fuel power plants in Switzerland, such that nuclear power has come to be perceived as a cheap, green alternative. (28) These factors resulted in a more favorable public opinion about nuclear power in Switzerland and elsewhere before the accident in Fukushima. Whereas only 49% of the US population favored the use of nuclear power for electricity generation in 1983, this percentage increased to 74% in (29) Similarly, the number of Europeans who accept nuclear power seemed to have increased lately. Within the 27 EU member states, for example, the number of people supporting the expansion of nuclear power has recently increased (from 14% in 2006 to 17% in 2009), whereas the percentage of Europeans opposing nuclear power had decreased (from 39% in 2006

9 9 to 34% in 2009). (30) Since people s attitudes recently have been more in favor of using nuclear power compared to those in the 1970s and 1980s, a nuclear catastrophe may have less influence on the public s opinion of nuclear power compared to 25 years ago. It could be that although people do recognize the risks and negative consequences, the perceived benefits for the climate and for a secure energy supply are still the main determinants of people s acceptance. The nuclear accident in Fukushima provided an opportunity to examine this question. 2. RATIONALE AND MODEL The general aim of our study was to investigate the effect of the nuclear accident in Fukushima on people s perception and acceptance of nuclear energy in Switzerland. More specifically, we examined how this event affected the degree to which trust, perceived benefits, and perceived risks influenced people s acceptance of nuclear power stations. Thus, our main construct under investigation was people s acceptance of nuclear power stations before and after the accident in Japan. Therefore, acceptance of nuclear power stations before and after Fukushima were the latent variables to be explained in the model, which is presented in Fig.1. Previous research suggests that both risk and benefit perceptions determine acceptance of nuclear power. (27, 31) Furthermore, people s perceived benefits of a secure energy supply seem to have a stronger influence on acceptance than do perceived risks. (27) Little is known, however, about the stability of risk and benefit perceptions over time (see also refs. 2, 23). Therefore, it is unknown how a severe accident changes the impact of risk and benefit perceptions on acceptance of nuclear power. After a nuclear accident, perceived risks may be more important to acceptance of nuclear power compared to perceived benefits. In line with this explanation, Hughey and Sundstrom (21) found that after the accident at TMI, laypeople s acceptance of a new local nuclear

10 10 power plant was determined mainly by their hazard expectations and less by the expected benefits. We hypothesized that in 2010, before the nuclear accident, perceived risks of nuclear power would have a small, negative effect on acceptance (path a in Fig. 1), while perceived benefits would more strongly influence acceptance of this energy source (path b in Fig. 1). We further expected, as in the case of gene technology, (32, 33) that there would exist a causal path from perceived benefits to perceived risks (path c in Fig. 1). Perceived benefits are thought to influence perceived risks, and not the other way around, because the benefits of nuclear power, such as energy production and the lack of CO 2 emissions, are concrete and known. The benefits, therefore, result in a clear evaluation. The risks, though, are mostly vague and difficult to imagine for laypeople. For example, many people do not know the exact impact and probability of radioactive leakage. This unclear idea regarding the risks makes their perception easy to modify for cases in which they have conflicting beliefs about nuclear power. (34) That is, if people perceive that there are many benefits to nuclear power, they can easily reduce their perception of risks so that the conflict is solved. (Insert Fig. 1 about here) People seem to rely on social trust to assess risks and benefits if they have little knowledge of the technology or hazard. (35) They then assume that their level of trust in specific actors will provide a good indication of the risks and benefits of the technology. (36) Trust in the actors managing a hazard or technology has been found to be an important determinant of the perceived risks and benefits of various technologies and hazards, (36, 37) as well as that of nuclear energy. (17, 27, 37-40) Laypeople reported having relatively little knowledge of nuclear power in previous studies. (35, 41) Therefore, we hypothesized that in 2010, trust in the actors involved in nuclear

11 11 power was negatively related to perceived risks (path d in Fig. 1) and positively related to perceived benefits (path e in Fig. 1). After the accident in 2011, we expected a similar relation between risk perception, benefit perception, and acceptance of nuclear power stations. Thus, the same model explains laypeople s acceptance of nuclear power after the accident (paths f, g, and h in Fig. 1). Trust is a crucial variable that determines people s interpretation such accidents. (35, 42) Thus, we hypothesized that in 2011, trust would be important for determining risk perception (path i in Fig. 1) and benefit perception (path j in Fig. 1) because people rely on trust to interpret the accident. (43) In other words, for determining trust, value information seems to be more important than performance information. (43) Because values are relatively stable within individuals, we expected that trust in 2011 is strongly related to trust in 2010 (path k in Fig. 1), although the performance information may have reduced people s trust in the nuclear power industry and regulatory agencies after the nuclear accident. As shown in Fig. 1, we further presumed that risk and benefit perceptions, as well as acceptance, are not correlated across the two measurement points after controlling for trust. Besides testing the model presented in Fig. 1, we examined the mean changes of the main constructs included in the model. Based on the studies done after the two previous nuclear accidents, (2-4, 10) we expected that people s acceptance of nuclear power stations in Switzerland, their benefit perceptions, and their trust would decrease after the Fukushima nuclear accident, whereas perceived risks would increase. 3. METHOD 3.1. Procedure and Sample Data were collected in two waves. The first survey was conducted more than five months

12 12 before the earthquake and tsunami in Japan, at the end of September We sent a paper-andpencil questionnaire together with a stamped and addressed return envelope to a random sample of households in the German-speaking part of Switzerland. A computer program had randomly selected the sample from telephone directories. After four weeks, a reminder was sent to the addresses from which we had not received a completed survey. This was repeated after three weeks. In the end, we received 1,233 questionnaires (a response rate of 40%). The same group of respondents was asked to fill out a shorter version of the previous questionnaire two weeks after the catastrophic event, at the end of March This second survey was sent to the 1,233 addresses from the first survey. We sent one reminder to non-respondents after three weeks. In total, 929 questionnaires were returned (a response rate of 77%). In the instructions for the second survey, recipients were explicitly informed that the person who completed the first survey should also fill in the new survey. We reminded the respondents that their data from the previous survey and those of the new survey were anonymous and that only anonymized datasets were linked. We asked for each respondent s birth year and gender in order to match the datasets of the two surveys. Datasets with different birth years or genders were deleted. After omitting cases missing more than 20% of their data as well as cases with two or more missing data regarding the dependent variable acceptance of nuclear power stations, the dataset included 790 respondents, from which the analyses described in this paper were done Questionnaire The 2010 survey was about people s perceptions of nuclear power stations in Switzerland and included items regarding ten different constructs and demographics. Four constructs are of importance here: acceptance of nuclear power stations, risk perception, benefit perception, and trust. Each construct was measured with three items (see Table I for the items, their mean scores,

13 13 and their standard deviations). The items were assessed on 7-point Likert scales, with higher scores indicating higher values on the construct measured. The shorter questionnaire, filled out after the accident in Japan, contained the same items in order to assess acceptance of nuclear power, risk perception, benefit perception, and trust. In addition, we included two items that assessed respondents perceived similarity between the situations in Japan and Switzerland regarding nuclear accidents (e.g., It is very likely that a similar nuclear accident could happen in Switzerland ). (Insert Table I about here) 3.3. Data Analysis To be able to apply structural equation modeling (SEM) to our data, we had to replace any missing data in our data set. Forty-six respondents (5.8%) did not answer all of the survey questions that were identical in the two waves (24 items; see Table 1). Of the respondents with missing data, most of them missed one item (3.4% of the sample) or three items (1.3% of the sample). Using SPSS 18.0, we applied the Expectation-Maximization (EM) algorithm, based on the maximum likelihood approach, to replace missing values. The values of the items in the respective construct were used to estimate the missing values. We first tested the explanatory model of acceptance of nuclear power in 2010 and the explanatory model of acceptance of nuclear power in We then tested the full model presented in Fig. 1. All models were analyzed using structural equation modeling (SEM). SEM is a statistical analysis procedure that tests the fit between a hypothesized model and a data set. It combines confirmatory factor analysis, used to validate the measurements of latent factors, with regression analysis, which examines the paths between latent factors. Moreover, SEM can simultaneously examine several structural relations between latent constructs. Parameters were

14 14 estimated using the maximum likelihood method in the Analysis of Moment Structures program (AMOS) version We conducted a confirmatory factor analysis to check the quality of the constructs. All factor loadings (λs) were substantial (λs >.67) and significant, given that the critical ratios of all variables (i.e., the variable estimate divided by its standard error) were larger than 1.96 (ps < 0.05). The items thus represented their constructs well enough to be included in our model. In the full model, we correlated the error terms corresponding to the identical latent-factor measurements across time in order to get reliable estimates of the regression paths. (44) Error terms for the same measurements namely tend to correlate when they are assessed among the same sample across time, which is, of course, the case in our longitudinal study. For example, the measurement error of an item that belongs to the latent factor perceived risks in 2010 is related to the measurement error of the identical item in The factor loadings and the correlations between the error terms of the indicator variables are, for the sake of simplicity, not shown in Figures 1 and 3. Because our sample was relatively large, significant chi-square tests were insufficient for assessing the model s goodness of fit. Therefore, we used the comparative fit index (CFI), the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) as criteria for our model. (45) All three statistics have value ranges between 0 and 1. A large SRMR indicates a poorly fitting model, whereas a SRMR smaller than.08 signifies a model that fits well to the data. For the RMSEA, a value equal to or smaller than.06 indicates a close fit. A CFI value equal to or over.95 indicates a good fit (see ref. 45 for an overview of the fit indices and their cut-off values). All these fit indices are descriptive; they are based not on inferential statistics but on rules of thumb to determine goodness of fit.

15 15 Additionally, we looked at the theoretical background of the model as a criterion for the fit of the model. (46) To see whether the model could be improved by including other relations between the constructs, we compared the theoretical background and the modification indices (MIs) with their respective expected parameter change (EPC). The values of the MIs indicate whether relations between certain items, latent constructs, and error terms could be included as these are likely to improve the model fit. We used the difference in χ² to test the improvement of the new model. 4. RESULTS 4.1. Respondents The mean age of our final sample of 790 respondents was years (SD = 15.02), and 67.2% (n = 531) of the respondents were male. Most respondents had finished vocational school (n = 324, 41.7%) or college or university (n = 242, 31%). Almost 19% of the sample had completed up to a higher secondary school level (n = 147), and 8.5% had only finished the country s obligatory school levels (n = 66). Our sample was older, included more men, more people with a college or university degree, and fewer people who had only finished obligatory (47, 48) school than was representative of the average Swiss population. We compared the respondents who participated in both surveys to those who had only completed the survey in 2010 (i.e., the non-respondents in the 2011 survey). Of the 304 nonrespondents, 287 cases remained after deleting those missing more than 20% of the 2010 data. The gender distribution was similar among respondents and non-respondents (χ²(1) =.12, p =.73). Non-respondents were slightly younger (M = 53.55, SD = 16.37) than respondents (M = 57.15, SD = 15.02), t(1,071) = -3.37, p =.001, r =.10. Also, respondents had a somewhat higher education level (18.6% with a higher-secondary-school education and 30.6% with a college- or university-level education) than non-respondents (13.6% with a higher-secondary-school

16 16 education and 26.1% with a college- or university-level education), U = 98,079.50, p =.01, r = Non-respondents showed more acceptance of nuclear power (M = 4.55, SD = 1.77) than respondents (M = 4.26, SD = 1.95), t(555.49) = 2.25, p =.03, r =.10. Non-respondents also perceived fewer risks from this technology (M = 3.57, SD = 1.47) than did respondents (M = 3.84, SD = 1.66), t(539.67) = -2.59, p =.01, r =.11. The respondents and non-respondents did not differ regarding perceived benefits and trust, ts < 1.77, ps > Acceptance of Nuclear Power Stations, Perceived Risks, Perceived Benefits, and Trust before and after Fukushima As can be seen in Table II, respondents acceptance of nuclear power stations in 2010 was around the midpoint of the 7-point rating scale, and it significantly decreased after the nuclear accident. Both perceived benefits and trust decreased significantly after the events in Japan. Respondents level of risk perception significantly increased after the nuclear accident. However, the sizes of the differences (i.e.,.79 at maximum for perceived risks, measured on 7-point Likert scales) between the two surveys indicate that respondents had not completely changed their views on nuclear power. Moreover, respondents saw the situation in Japan regarding nuclear power stations as being relatively similar to the situation in Switzerland, given that the mean perceived similarity was slightly above the midpoint of the 7-point scale (M = 4.54, SD = 1.85). (Insert Table II about here) 4.3. Separate Models for Explaining Acceptance of Nuclear Power Stations before and after the Nuclear Accident The model for explaining acceptance of nuclear power stations before the nuclear accident fit quite well to our data (χ²(49) = , p =.0001, CFI =.984, SRMR =.027, RMSEA =.052). The standardized regression weights of the paths were all significant and in the hypothesized

17 17 directions (ps <.001). However, the modification index (MI) between the two error terms of the items trust in science and trust in the Federal Office of Energy (FOE) in 2010 indicated that including the correlation between these two error terms in our model would result in a substantial improvement of our model fit. People probably see scientists and the FOE as independent actors with respect to nuclear power stations, and their independence may have a large role in people s trust evaluation of both actors; hence, our respondents rated them similarly. It therefore made sense to include the correlation between the two error terms in our 2010 model. As a result, the model fit indeed improved (χ²(48) = , p =.0001, CFI =.990, SRMR =.019, RMSEA =.041), which was found to be statistically significant (Δχ²(1) = 41.81, p < 0.001). All paths were significant (p <.001) and had similar directions and sizes of influence compared to our initial model, described above. Fig. 2 presents the final 2010 model, including the standardized coefficients (in regular font) but without the factor loadings. In 2010, perceived risks and perceived benefits explained 90% of the variance of acceptance of nuclear power stations. Perceived benefits had the largest influence on acceptance, and risk perception had a smaller negative relation to acceptance. Trust had a strong positive influence on perceived benefits in 2010 and a negative influence on perceived risks. As well, perceived benefits had a negative influence on perceived risks. The squared multiple correlations (SMCs) were substantial for perceived benefits (SMC =.59) and perceived risks (SMC =.79). (Insert Fig. 2 about here) The same model also explained well the data collected after the nuclear accident in Japan (χ²(49) = , p =.0001, CFI =.970, SRMR =.033, RMSEA =.075). The MIs again indicated that the correlation between the error terms of the items trust in science and trust in the FOE in 2011 would increase the model fit. We therefore tested a new model that included the correlation

18 18 between these two error terms. This, too, improved the model fit significantly (χ²(48) = , p =.0001, CFI =.979, SRMR =.026, RMSEA =.064, Δχ²(1) = 67.11, p < 0.001). In 2011, perceived benefits and perceived risks also largely contributed to the explained variance of acceptance of nuclear power stations (SMC =.94). Perceived benefits again had a strong positive influence on acceptance (see Fig. 2, standardized coefficients in bold font), whereas perceived risks had a smaller, negative effect on acceptance. Trust had a positive influence on perceived benefits and had a negative influence on perceived risks. Perceived benefits contributed negatively to perceived risks. The explained variances were again substantial for perceived benefits (SMC =.66) and perceived risks (SMC =.85) The Full Model In our SEM analysis of the full model (Fig. 1), we immediately included the correlations between the error terms of the items trust in the FOE and trust in science in 2010 and The hypothesized model fit quite well to our data (χ²(227) = , p =.0001, CFI =.968, SRMR =.053, RMSEA =.052). The standardized regression weights of the paths were all significant and in the hypothesized directions (ps <.001). The MIs suggested that including the path between perceived benefits in 2010 and perceived benefits in 2011 would substantially improve the model. This inclusion also made theoretical sense because the benefits of nuclear power remained the same; the nuclear accident had not affected them. People therefore relied on the perceived benefits before the nuclear accident in order to estimate the benefits after such an event. Including the path between the perceived benefits in 2010 and 2011 did result in a model that fit better to the data (χ²(226) = , p =.0001, CFI =.980, SRMR =.031, RMSEA =.042) and was found to be statistically significant (Δχ²(1) = , p < ). The combination of the explanatory models for 2010 and 2011 explained 95% of the variance regarding acceptance of

19 19 nuclear power plants in The SMCs of acceptance in 2010, trust in 2011, and perceived risks and perceived benefits in 2010 and 2011 were substantial (.61 < SMC <.92). The MIs did not suggest including any other relations that would substantially improve the model fit. Table I presents the standardized factor loadings of the individual items on the latent constructs in the final model. The correlations between the error terms corresponding to the identical latent factor measurements in 2010 and in 2011 ranged between.04 and.34. The direction of the paths described in the separate models remained the same in the full model (see Fig. 3). Additionally, perceived benefits in 2010 had a positive influence on perceived benefits in Similarly, trust in 2010 was strongly related to trust in When comparing the regression path between perceived risks and acceptance in 2010 to that of 2011, it looks as if perceived risks had a stronger influence on acceptance after the nuclear accident compared to before. We checked whether these paths were indeed significantly different by, first, restraining the two paths between perceived risks and acceptance in 2010 and in 2011 in a new model and then comparing the quality of the new model to that of the unrestrained model. (49) If the influence of risk perception on acceptance in 2011 was indeed larger than that in 2010, fixing these two paths should result in a significantly worse-fitting model. The new model with the two restrained paths had a good fit to the data (χ²(227) = , p =.0001, CFI =.980, SRMR =.031, RMSEA =.042), but the restrained model did not result in a statistically significantly worse-fitting (or better-fitting) model compared to the unrestrained model (Δχ²(1) = 1.92, p =.17). This result seems to indicate that the impact of risk perception on acceptance in 2010 was already similar to that in 2011 in the unrestrained version of the full model. (Insert Fig. 3 about here) In sum, 95% of the variance in our respondents acceptance of nuclear power stations after

20 20 the accident in Fukushima could be explained by their risk perception and benefit perception after the accident. The model suggests that there is both a direct effect of trust in 2011 and an indirect effect of trust in 2010 on perceived risks and benefits in Both before and after the nuclear accident, perceived benefits had the strongest influence on acceptance of nuclear power stations, compared to perceived risks. 5. DISCUSSION There is a lack of longitudinal studies in the domain of risk-perception research. Therefore, little is known about how technological accidents influence people s risk perception and acceptance of various technologies. The accident at Fukushima provided an opportunity to conduct a quasi-experimental study. A random sample of laypeople who participated in a survey about nuclear power in autumn 2010 answered the same set of questions about trust, perceived risks and benefits, and acceptance of nuclear power after the accident in Japan. In our model, we predicted that trust is a key factor, one that directly influences risk perception and benefit perception and that indirectly influences acceptance of nuclear power. Furthermore, we hypothesized that trust before the accident had a significant effect on trust after the accident. Our model explained the data very well. In the following paragraphs, we will first discuss the effect of the nuclear accident on people s perceptions and acceptance of nuclear power. We will then elaborate further on the role of trust concerning the acceptance of nuclear power stations. We will also suggest a few implications of our findings for risk communication about nuclear power stations. Lastly, we will provide some critical remarks about our study. To recap, our respondents reported higher levels of risk perception and lower levels of trust, perceived benefits, and acceptance regarding nuclear power plants after the nuclear accident

21 21 compared to before. Our findings, thus, seem to indicate that the nuclear accident in Fukushima resulted in a more negative perception of nuclear power and, hence, had important implications for the image of this energy technology among the public, at least in the short term. However, the influences of the determinants of acceptance risk perception, benefit perception, and trust seemed to remain stable over time. It is therefore very likely that because perceived benefits and trust decreased and perceived risks similarly increased, the acceptance of nuclear power plants also decreased. Past studies have shown that for gene technology, (32, 33) nanotechnology, (50) and nuclear power, (27) perceived benefits have a stronger impact on acceptance of the respective technology than do perceived risks. The present study not only appears to replicate this finding but also suggests that after a severe accident, the importance of perceived risks increases somewhat, although perceived benefits remain a more important predictor of acceptance. Thus, people may consider nuclear power s benefit for the energy supply and for a healthy economy to be more important to their acceptance of it than the possible risks that are associated with it. It may come as a surprise that risk perception appeared to have a smaller influence as benefit perception on acceptance of nuclear power stations after learning about the nuclear accident. Studies conducted after Chernobyl and TMI pointed toward the importance of risk perception on attitudes toward nuclear power. (20, 21) In our model, we were able to compare the influence of risk perception on acceptance to that of benefit perception, which showed that the impact of perceived risks had not changed much after the Fukushima accident and that perceived benefits remained the strongest predictor of acceptance. An explanation for these findings could be that our respondents had already acknowledged the risks of nuclear power before the nuclear accident and they therefore did not differ much in their risk perception both before and after the nuclear

22 22 accident. Alternatively, our respondents varied more regarding their benefit perception. Perceived benefits could therefore make a larger contribution toward explaining acceptance than could perceived risks. This finding is supported by findings from previous studies. Proponents of nuclear power have been found to be more ambivalent about this technology, seeing both its benefits and risks, and therefore have a less stable attitude toward nuclear power than opponents. (23) Moreover, people have been found either to have both positive and negative images about nuclear power or to have only negative images, but hardly any person reports only positive images. (51) Even proponents seem to acknowledge some risks of nuclear power. (25) Next to perceived benefits, social trust continued to be an important determinant in our full model. It influenced risk and benefit perceptions directly and, therefore, acceptance of the technology, though indirectly. Also, the high correlation between trust in 2010 and trust in 2011 seems to confirm the fundamental role of trust in risk perception, benefit perception, and acceptance. After the nuclear accident, the direct influence of trust on perceived benefits appeared smaller than before the nuclear accident. This was caused by the additional path between the perceived benefits at the two measurement times; thus, trust in 2010 not only influenced perceived benefits in 2011 through trust in 2011 but also through the connection between perceived benefits in 2010 and perceived benefits in These findings support the idea that trust remains fundamental to risk and benefit perceptions, even when new information about the hazard is available. The importance of trust is also in line with the Trust, Confidence, (36, 42) and Cooperation (TCC) model, which distinguishes between trust and confidence. According to this model, confidence is determined by past performance and competence, whereas social trust is determined by value similarity. The disaster in Japan, which can be viewed as past performance information, reduced people s trust. The reduction was rather small, however, given

23 23 the severity of the accident, which may imply that it is not performance information but value similarity that influenced people s trust. The second survey took place right after the accident. Future studies are needed in order to examine changes in trust over time. It is possible that the more time that passes after an accident, the less effect the accident will have on trust and, as a result, on acceptance of nuclear power. The findings of our longitudinal study seem to imply that even after a nuclear accident, perceived benefits remain the most important determinant of acceptance, compared to perceived risks. An implication of this finding for communication with the public after a nuclear accident may be that focusing on both the risks and benefits of nuclear power will change the public s acceptance of nuclear power. However, only concentrating on the risks will not significantly affect the public s opinion. In comparison, discussions about the benefits of nuclear power such as a secure energy supply or the lack of benefits, for that matter, are more likely to change people s acceptance of nuclear power. (27) Politicians in various countries reacted in different ways to the accident in Japan. In France and the USA, politicians did not reconsider nuclear power, whereas in Germany and Switzerland, politicians quickly abandoned their support of nuclear power. The public s change in opinion after the accident, however, did not appear to be extreme. As in previous studies, (3, 4) our respondents did not seem to change their attitudes about nuclear power drastically. Therefore, politicians should not react too quickly by changing nuclear policy on behalf of the public. Our study was a longitudinal survey, so we had the benefit of being able to follow changes in the perception and acceptance of nuclear power stations within the same sample of people. Our study, however, also had a few limitations. The first was that the second survey was sent out two weeks after the earthquake and tsunami in Japan. People may not have been able to form a firm

24 24 opinion about nuclear power stations so quickly after the event. The situation at the power plants in Fukushima was not stable at that time, so laypeople may have had the idea that a catastrophic event such as an explosion was still likely to occur or, conversely, that the problems in the reactors would soon be solved. This uncertainty may have resulted in attitudes and beliefs that are more negative or unstable than if the situation had been stabilized. If we had postponed the second measurement for a few weeks, our results may have been different (cf. ref. 3). Consequently, it would be interesting to follow our sample over time to examine these people s perceptions of nuclear power after the accident has ceased being in the news. It can be speculated whether and how public perception changes in the long term. It could be that the public s view of nuclear power remains more negative than it was before the accident, as was found after the Chernobyl accident in a Dutch study. (3) Verplanken suggested that due to this accident, the public was better able to imagine the occurrence of extremely unlikely events, such that the perceived risks of nuclear power remained high after Chernobyl. Alternatively, the public could return to the view of nuclear power that it had held before the Fukushima accident. Media coverage about the accident became less intense: the number of hits for the term Fukushima in the Swiss version of Google News was reduced to 612 hits six months after the event compared to about 11,500 hits four weeks after the accident. This reduction could have limited people s access to mental images about nuclear accidents (cf. ref. 3). Also, Swiss people may have readjusted their attitudes a few months after the accident by realizing that the physical consequences of the accident had happened far away and that the local situation was safe. (52) This latter effect may, of course, only apply to populations not directly affected by the nuclear accident in Fukushima. Another critical remark about our study is that we may not have included all relevant

25 25 determinants of nuclear power acceptance. For example, attitudes toward nuclear power have also been found to be related to fairness (15) and to concerns about climate change and the environment. (18) Also, our trust measure may not have comprised all relevant actors. As we wanted to investigate the role of social trust in our model, we assessed trust in three specific actors that are relevant for the management of nuclear power stations in Switzerland: the operators of the nuclear power stations, nuclear scientists, and the Swiss Federal Office of Energy (FOE). Nevertheless, the high factor loadings of all three items both before and after the nuclear accident seem to indicate that the three items are highly related and that they provide a good representation of the social trust construct. Similarly, we cannot explain the underlying cause of the decrease in public acceptance of nuclear power and its related concepts. As was the case regarding the previous nuclear accidents, media coverage was high, (53) and the public s view of this technology became more negative directly after the events in Fukushima. (3, 4) The study did not allow for an investigation of the influence of media on the effects we found. More research is therefore needed to fully understand what caused the increase in a negative view of nuclear power after the nuclear accident at Fukushima. An advantage of our study was that we could investigate changes in the perception of nuclear power within the same subjects. However, a disadvantage of our longitudinal survey was that we lost a number of respondents in the second wave. Consequently, our sample became older and better educated. Moreover, for some points, respondents views about nuclear power were less positive than those of non-respondents. In short, it could be that our final sample was less representative of the Swiss population than our initial sample, which may have affected our outcomes. When we compared the 2010 model that included the initial sample to the 2010 model

26 26 that included only respondents who had completed both surveys, the directions and strengths of the paths, as well as the explained variances, were similar in both models. Furthermore, because our final sample was more critical of nuclear power before Fukushima than was the complete 2010 sample, we may have underestimated the relations in our model. Particularly, we would expect that the influence of perceived benefits on acceptance of nuclear power stations to be larger, whereas the influence of perceived risks on acceptance would be smaller in a more representative sample, i.e., a sample with more respondents who were positive about nuclear power before the nuclear accident. One could criticize that the study was conducted in Switzerland. Switzerland is, of course, quite far from Japan. It may be that people in countries closer to Japan responded differently to the nuclear accident. Moreover, the Swiss energy situation may be different from that of other countries such that the survey results cannot be generalized to other countries. Switzerland depends on nuclear power stations for about 40% of its power generation. Three of the currently running reactors should be replaced by 2020 to maintain the same energy supply. By the time we conducted the second survey, the decision to postpone all applications for new nuclear power reactors had just been made (March 14, 2011). However, our finding that benefit perception mainly determined people s acceptance of nuclear power stations, both before and after the nuclear accident, is in line with the results of previous studies that showed the overwhelming influence of benefit perception on acceptance of other technologies. (31, 33) Therefore, we believe that our results can be generalized to other countries. In sum, our study is one of the few longitudinal studies that investigated the public s acceptance of nuclear power before and after a nuclear accident. We believe that it represents a major contribution to the limited knowledge available about this subject. After the nuclear

27 27 accident, the public s acceptance and perceptions of nuclear power stations as well as its trust in the stakeholders had become more negative. Nevertheless, our explanatory model did not show a shift in the importance of the relations between the constructs that explained people s acceptance of nuclear power stations. Therefore, a nuclear accident, although catastrophic for the people living near the plant and for the environment, does not appear to result in radical changes in the influences of perceived benefits, perceived risks, or trust on the acceptance of this technology. ACKNOWLEDGEMENT The first survey reported in this paper (conducted in 2010) was funded by Swissnuclear. Swissnuclear comprises representatives of the Swiss electric supply companies (Alpiq, Axpo, BKW, CKW and EGL), which are committed to the safe and economic operation of the nuclear power plants in Switzerland.

28 28 REFERENCES 1. IAEA. Fukushima Nuclear Accident Update Log, Available at: org/newscenter/news/2011/fukushimafull.html, accessed on April 13, Lindell MK, Perry RW. Effects of the Chernobyl accident on public perceptions of nuclear plant accident risks. Risk Analysis, 1990; 10(3): Verplanken B. Beliefs, attitudes, and intentions toward nuclear energy before and after Chernobyl in a longitudinal within-subjects design. Environment and Behavior, 1989; 21(4): Eiser JR, Spears R, Webley P. Nuclear attitudes before and after Chernobyl: Change and judgment. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 1989; 19(8): van der Pligt J. Public attitudes to nuclear energy: Salience and anxiety. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 1985; 5(1): McDaniels TL. Chernobyl's effects on the perceived risks of nuclear power: A small sample test. Risk Analysis, 1988; 8(3): Peters HP, Albrecht G, Hennen L et al. 'Chernobyl' and the nuclear power issue in West German public opinion. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 1990; 10(2): Eiser JR, Hannover B, Mann L et al. Nuclear attitudes after Chernobyl: A cross-national study. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 1990; 10(2): Renn O. Public responses to the Chernobyl accident. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 1990; 10(2): Rosa EA, Dunlap RE. Nuclear power: Three decades of public opinion. Public Opinion Quarterly, 1994; 58(2): de Boer C, Catsburg I. The impact of nuclear accidents on attitudes toward nuclear

29 29 energy. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 1988; 52(2): Bolsen T, Cook FL. Public opinion on energy policy: Public Opinion Quarterly, 2008; 72(2): Eiser JR, van der Pligt J. Beliefs and values in the nuclear debate. Journal of Apllied Social Psychology, 1979; 9: Pidgeon NF, Lorenzoni I, Poortinga W. Climate change or nuclear power No thanks! A quantitative study of public perceptions and risk framing in Britain. Global Environmental Change, 2008; 18(1): Besley JC. Does fairness matter in the context of anger about nuclear energy decision making? Risk Analysis, 2012; 32(1): Greenberg M, Truelove HB. Energy choices and risk beliefs: Is it just global warming and fear of a nuclear power plant accident? Risk Analysis, 2011; 31(5): Greenberg M. Energy sources, public policy, and public preferences: Analysis of US national and site-specific data. Energy Policy, 2009; 37(8): Spence A, Poortinga W, Pidgeon N et al. Public perceptions of energy choices: The influence of beliefs about climate change and the environment. Energy & Environment, 2010; 21(5): van der Pligt J, Midden CJH. Chernobyl: Four years later: Attitudes, risk management and communication. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 1990; 10: Drottz-Sjöberg B-M, Sjoberg L. Risk perception and worries after the Chernobyl accident. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 1990; 10(2): Hughey JB, Sundstrom E. Perceptions of Three Mile Island and acceptance of a nuclear power plant in a distant community. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 1988;

30 30 18(10): Menard S. Longitudinal Research. 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage University Paper; Midden CJH, Verplanken B. The stability of nuclear attitudes after Chernobyl. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 1990; 10(2): World Nuclear Association. The Nuclear Renaissance, Available at: accessed on May 5, Keller C, Visschers V, Siegrist M. Affective Imagery and Acceptance of Replacing Nuclear Power Plants. Risk Analysis, 2012; 32 (3): European Commission. Attitudes towards Radioactive Waste. Brussels: TNS Opinion & Social: Special Eurobarometer 297 / Wave Visschers VHM, Keller C, Siegrist M. Climate change benefits and energy supply benefits as determinants of acceptance of nuclear power stations: Investigating an explanatory model. Energy Policy, 2011; 39(6): Roth S, Hirschberg S, Bauer C et al. Sustainability of electricity supply technology portfolio. Annals of Nuclear Energy, 2009; 36(3): Bisconti Research Inc. Public Support for Nuclear Energy at Record High, March Available at: accessed on January 11, European Commission. Europeans and Nuclear Safety. Brussels: TNS Opinion & Social: Special Eurobarometer 324 / Wave Tanaka Y. Major psychological factors determining public acceptance of the siting of nuclear facilities. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 2004; 34(6):

31 Siegrist M. The influence of trust and perceptions of risks and benefits on the acceptance of gene technology. Risk Analysis, 2000; 20: Siegrist M. A causal model explaining the perception and acceptance of gene technology. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 1999; 29: Abelson RP. Modes of resolution of belief dilemmas. The Journal of Conflict Resolution, 1959; 3(4): Siegrist M, Cvetkovich G. Perception of hazards: The role of social trust and knowledge. Risk Analysis, 2000; 20 (5): Earle TC, Siegrist M, Gutscher H. Trust, risk perception, and the TCC model of cooperation. Pp.1-49 in Siegrist M, Earle TC, Gutscher H (eds). Trust in cooperative risk management: Uncertainty and scepticism in the public mind. London: Earthscan; Siegrist M, Cvetkovich G, Roth C. Salient value similarity, social trust, and risk/benefit perception. Risk Analysis, 2000; 20(3): Flynn J, Burns W, Mertz CK et al. Trust as a determinant of opposition to a high-level radioactive waste repository: Analysis of a structural model. Risk Analysis, 1992; 12(3): Viklund MJ. Trust and risk perception in Western Europe: A cross-national study. Risk Analysis, 2003; 23(4): Whitfield SC, Rosa EA, Dan A et al. The future of nuclear power: Value orientations and risk perception. Risk Analysis, 2009; 29(3): European Commission. Europeans and Nuclear Safety. Brussels: TNS Opinion & Social: Special Eurobarometer 271 / Wave Earle TC. Trust in risk management: A model-based review of empirical research. Risk

32 32 Analysis, 2010; 30(4): Earle TC, Siegrist M. Morality information, performance information, and the distrinction between trust and confidence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 2006; 36(2): Jöreskog KG. Statistical models and methods for analysis of longitudinal data. Pp in Jöreskog KG, Sörbom D (eds). Advances in Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Models. Cambridge, MA: Abt Books; Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 1999; 6(1): Iacobucci D. Everything you always wanted to know about SEM (structural equations modeling) but were afraid to ask. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 2009; 19(4): Swiss Statistics. Bildungsstand der Wohnbevölkerung nach Alter und Geschlecht [Education level of the resident population according to age and gender] Available at: html?open=9#9, accessed on May 31, Swiss Statistics. Bevölkerungsstand und -struktur [Population size and composition], Available at: 01.html, accessed on May 31, Byrne BM. Structural Equation Modeling with AMOS: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming. Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; Siegrist M, Cousin M-E, Kastenholz H et al. Public acceptance of nanotechnology foods and food packaging: The influence of affect and trust. Appetite, 2007; 49(2):

33 Peters E, Slovic P. The role of affect and worldviews as orienting dispositions in the perception and acceptance of nuclear power. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 1996; 26(16): Butler C, Parkhill KA, Pidgeon NF. Nuclear power after Japan: The social dimensions. Environment: Science and Policy for Sustainable Development, 2011; 53(6): Friedman SM. Three Mile Island, Chernobyl, and Fukushima: An analysis of traditional and new media coverage of nuclear accidents and radiation. Bulletin of the Atomic Scientists, 2011; 67(5):55-65.

34 34 Table I. Questionnaire Items per Scale in 2010 and 2011, Including their Mean Values (and Standard Deviations) and Standardized Factor Loadings in the Final Model M (SD) Standardized M (SD) Standardized Items per scale a Acceptance factor loadings factor loadings 1. Switzerland needs a lot of electricity; people should therefore accept nuclear 4.05 (2.25) (2.12).92 power stations. 2. Switzerland can renounce nuclear power stations without any problems. b 4.59 (2.13) (2.11) We need nuclear power stations in Switzerland because renewable energy 4.15 (2.21) (2.14).88 sources alone do not produce sufficient electricity. Perceived risks 4. I am very concerned about the dangers of nuclear power stations in 4.12 (1.98) (2.01).66 Switzerland. 5. The nuclear power stations that are built now or in the near future are safe. b 3.75 (1.89) (2.00) The catastrophe risks in new nuclear power stations are very small. b 3.63 (1.88) (2.01).85

35 M (SD) Standardized M (SD) Standardized Items per scale a Perceived Benefits factor loadings factor loadings 7. New nuclear power stations will protect Switzerland from an electricity 4.63 (2.06) (2.11).80 bottleneck. 8. The electricity price would become too high in Switzerland if the existing 4.24 (1.94) (1.99).77 nuclear power stations were not be replaced. 9. Industry would still have a secured energy supply without replacing the 4.24 (1.92) (1.97).80 existing nuclear power stations. b Trust 11. in scientists in the field of nuclear power (1.65) (1.77) in the Swiss Federal Office of Energy (1.58) (1.61) in the operators of nuclear power stations (2.00) (1.89).90 a All items were measured on 7-point Likert scales, with increasing scores indicating higher values on the item. b Item was recoded.

36 36 Table II. Means and Standard Deviations of the Four Constructs in 2010 and 2011, and Results of Paired-Samples t-tests between the Measurements in 2010 and M SD M SD t df Acceptance * 789 Perceived risks * 789 Perceived benefits * 789 Trust * 778 * p <.001. Note. All items measured on 7-point Likert scales.

37 37 FIGURE CAPTIONS. Fig. 1. Our Hypothesized Explanatory Model of Laypeople s Acceptance of Nuclear Power Stations before and after the Fukushima Disaster. Fig. 2. Results of Structural Equation Modeling Explaining Laypeople s Acceptance of Nuclear Power Stations before (Values in Regular Font) and after (Values in Bold Font) the Nuclear Accident in Japan, Including the Standardized Regression Weights and Squared Multiple Correlations (SMCs). Fig. 3. The Final Model Explaining Laypeople s Acceptance of Nuclear Power Stations before and after Fukushima, Including the Standardized Regression Weights and Squared Multiple Correlations (SMCs).

38 38

39 39 Note. All standardized regression weights are significant, p <.001. Standardized regression weights in regular font refer to data beforee the nuclear accident (2010), while values in bold font refer to data collected after the nuclear accident (2011).

40 Note. All standardized regression weights are significant, p <

A Proposal of Feed-in Tariff Personal Carbon Allowance (FIT-PCA) and its Evaluation

A Proposal of Feed-in Tariff Personal Carbon Allowance (FIT-PCA) and its Evaluation A Proposal of Feed-in Tariff Personal Carbon Allowance (FIT-PCA) and its Evaluation Takayoshi Kitamura 1, Asao Takamatsu 1, Hirotake Ishii 1 and Hiroshi Shimoda 1 1 Graduate School of Energy Science, Kyoto

More information

Attitudes towards radioactive waste in Switzerland Report

Attitudes towards radioactive waste in Switzerland Report Federal Department of the Environment, Transport, Energy and Communications DETEC Swiss Federal Office of Energy SFOE Disposal of Radioactive Waste Section Author: tns-opinion August 2008 Attitudes towards

More information

OREGON ELECTRICITY SURVEY

OREGON ELECTRICITY SURVEY OREGON ELECTRICITY SURVEY by Stephen M. Johnson, Ph.D., Associate Director with the assistance of Kimberlee Langolf January 1999 OREGON SURVEY RESEARCH LABORATORY UNIVERSITY OF OREGON EUGENE OR 97403-5245

More information

Kristin Gustavson * and Ingrid Borren

Kristin Gustavson * and Ingrid Borren Gustavson and Borren BMC Medical Research Methodology 2014, 14:133 RESEARCH ARTICLE Open Access Bias in the study of prediction of change: a Monte Carlo simulation study of the effects of selective attrition

More information

The GRS Emergency Centre during the Fukushima NPS Accident: Communicating Radiological Information to the Public

The GRS Emergency Centre during the Fukushima NPS Accident: Communicating Radiological Information to the Public The GRS Emergency Centre during the Fukushima NPS Accident: Communicating Radiological Information to the Public F.-N. Sentuc, S. Dokter Gesellschaft für Anlagen- und Reaktorsicherheit (GRS) mbh Schwertnergasse

More information

I reject your reality and substitute my own! Why more knowledge about CO 2 storage hardly improves public attitudes

I reject your reality and substitute my own! Why more knowledge about CO 2 storage hardly improves public attitudes Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Energy Procedia 00 (2013) 000 000 www.elsevier.com/locate/procedia GHGT-11 I reject your reality and substitute my own! Why more knowledge about CO 2 storage hardly

More information

2011 Iowa Biobased Consumer Survey

2011 Iowa Biobased Consumer Survey 2011 Iowa Biobased Consumer Survey M. House, Graduate Program Assistant, BioPreferred, Iowa State University Extension and Outreach B. Butcher, Undergraduate Assistant, BioPreferred, Iowa State University

More information

A Note on Sex, Geographic Mobility, and Career Advancement. By: William T. Markham, Patrick O. Macken, Charles M. Bonjean, Judy Corder

A Note on Sex, Geographic Mobility, and Career Advancement. By: William T. Markham, Patrick O. Macken, Charles M. Bonjean, Judy Corder A Note on Sex, Geographic Mobility, and Career Advancement By: William T. Markham, Patrick O. Macken, Charles M. Bonjean, Judy Corder This is a pre-copyedited, author-produced PDF of an article accepted

More information

Tailoring communication to the public s intuitive understanding of CCS Insights from experimental research in Switzerland

Tailoring communication to the public s intuitive understanding of CCS Insights from experimental research in Switzerland Tailoring communication to the public s intuitive understanding of CCS Insights from experimental research in Switzerland Lasse Wallquist, Selma L Orange, Simone Dohle, Michael Siegrist, ETH Zurich, Switzerland

More information

Presentation Outline. Basic Reactor Physics and Boiling Water Design Sequence of Events Consequences and Mitigation Conclusions and Lessons Learned

Presentation Outline. Basic Reactor Physics and Boiling Water Design Sequence of Events Consequences and Mitigation Conclusions and Lessons Learned Response of the Fukushima Dai-ichi Nuclear Plant to the March 11, 2011 Earthquake in Japan Dr. George Flanagan Oak Ridge National Laboratory Presented to the EERI/NEC Meeting April 12, 2012 Presentation

More information

NUMBERS, FACTS AND TRENDS SHAPING THE WORLD FOR RELEASE DECEMBER 8, 2014 FOR FURTHER INFORMATION ON THIS REPORT:

NUMBERS, FACTS AND TRENDS SHAPING THE WORLD FOR RELEASE DECEMBER 8, 2014 FOR FURTHER INFORMATION ON THIS REPORT: NUMBERS, FACTS AND TRENDS SHAPING THE WORLD FOR RELEASE DECEMBER 8, 2014 FOR FURTHER INFORMATION ON THIS REPORT: Kristen Purcell, Research Consultant Lee Rainie, Director, Internet, Science and Technology

More information

B ISCONTI R ESEARCH, INC WOODLEY P LACE NW WASHINGTON, DC T EL: FAX:

B ISCONTI R ESEARCH, INC WOODLEY P LACE NW WASHINGTON, DC T EL: FAX: B ISCONTI R ESEARCH, INC. 2610 WOODLEY P LACE NW WASHINGTON, DC 20008 T EL: 202.347.8820 FAX: 202.347.8825 http://www.bisconti.com Evolution of Public Opinion on Nuclear Energy In the United States Ann

More information

Communications In The Workplace

Communications In The Workplace 81 Chapter 6 Communications In The Workplace This chapter examines current levels of consultation, information and communication in the workplace. It outlines the type of information available in the workplace

More information

CHAPTER 5 DATA ANALYSIS AND RESULTS

CHAPTER 5 DATA ANALYSIS AND RESULTS 5.1 INTRODUCTION CHAPTER 5 DATA ANALYSIS AND RESULTS The purpose of this chapter is to present and discuss the results of data analysis. The study was conducted on 518 information technology professionals

More information

WHAT HAPPENED, WHAT IS GOING ON IN FUKUSHIMA NO.1 NUCLEAR POWER STATION?

WHAT HAPPENED, WHAT IS GOING ON IN FUKUSHIMA NO.1 NUCLEAR POWER STATION? WHAT HAPPENED, WHAT IS GOING ON IN FUKUSHIMA NO.1 NUCLEAR POWER STATION? Yoichiro Shimazu Research Institute of Nuclear Engineering University of Fukui WHERE IS FUKUSHIMA NUCLEAR POWER STATION? Sapporo

More information

respondents that it is their own personal or all landowners responsibility to protect water quality.

respondents that it is their own personal or all landowners responsibility to protect water quality. B); the questionnaire (Appendix A) with a cover letter (Appendix C), map, and selfaddressed, stamped return envelope; a reminder postcard (Appendix D); and a replacement questionnaire with cover letter

More information

CHAPTER 4 METHOD. procedures. It also describes the development of the questionnaires, the selection of the

CHAPTER 4 METHOD. procedures. It also describes the development of the questionnaires, the selection of the CHAPTER 4 METHOD 4.1 Introduction This chapter discusses the research design, sample, and data collection procedures. It also describes the development of the questionnaires, the selection of the research

More information

Contribution from Nuclear Engineering. Satoru Tanaka The University of Tokyo President, Atomic Energy Society of Japan

Contribution from Nuclear Engineering. Satoru Tanaka The University of Tokyo President, Atomic Energy Society of Japan Contribution from Nuclear Engineering Satoru Tanaka The University of Tokyo President, Atomic Energy Society of Japan Introduction Base on the insufficient actions as a nuclear energy scientist, I will

More information

Are innovative consumers prone to imitation perception? Developing a constructivist innovativeness model of imitation perception

Are innovative consumers prone to imitation perception? Developing a constructivist innovativeness model of imitation perception Are innovative consumers prone to imitation perception? Developing a constructivist innovativeness model of imitation perception Abstract Global firms desire to see that consumers perceive their firm and

More information

Abstract No A Study on Risk Perception toward Nuclear Power Operation in Taiwan

Abstract No A Study on Risk Perception toward Nuclear Power Operation in Taiwan Abstract No. 383 A Study on Risk Perception toward Nuclear Power Operation in Taiwan Ciao-tzu Lee, Shiang-ling Hu, Wushou P. Chang Institute of Public Health, and Institute of Environmental Health Sciences,

More information

The Center for Rural Studies 207 Morrill Hall University of Vermont Prepared by: Amy S. Hoskins, S. Helen Jordan, and Jane M. Kolodinsky, Ph.D.

The Center for Rural Studies 207 Morrill Hall University of Vermont Prepared by: Amy S. Hoskins, S. Helen Jordan, and Jane M. Kolodinsky, Ph.D. Vermonters Awareness, Knowledge and Opinions of Genetic Modification Vermonter Poll 2004 The Center for Rural Studies 207 Morrill Hall University of Vermont Prepared by: Amy S. Hoskins, S. Helen Jordan,

More information

An Analysis of Factors that Influence the Income of Poor Women: A Research Survey in Ho Chi Minh City of Vietnam

An Analysis of Factors that Influence the Income of Poor Women: A Research Survey in Ho Chi Minh City of Vietnam An Analysis of Factors that Influence the Income of Poor Women: A Research Survey in Ho Chi Minh City of Vietnam Do Phu Tran Tinh University of Economics and Law, Vietnam National Ho Chi Minh University,

More information

Which is the best way to measure job performance: Self-perceptions or official supervisor evaluations?

Which is the best way to measure job performance: Self-perceptions or official supervisor evaluations? Which is the best way to measure job performance: Self-perceptions or official supervisor evaluations? Ned Kock Full reference: Kock, N. (2017). Which is the best way to measure job performance: Self-perceptions

More information

Going Nuclear? The Multi-Level Politics of Nuclear Energy Conference Series

Going Nuclear? The Multi-Level Politics of Nuclear Energy Conference Series Going Nuclear? The Multi-Level Politics of Nuclear Energy Conference Series Seminar 2: Nuclear Energy in the UK Liverpool: November 1 st 2013 PI: Dr. Darren McCauley (Lecturer in Sustainable Development

More information

1 DEVELOPMENT OF EARTHQUAKE RISK MITIGATION IN SWITZERLAND

1 DEVELOPMENT OF EARTHQUAKE RISK MITIGATION IN SWITZERLAND State and development of earthquake risk mitigation In Switzerland Paper Title Line 3 B. Duvernay Federal Office for the Environment, Bern, Switzerland Blank Line 9 pt A.B. Author& C.D. Other (11pt) University

More information

COMMUNITY ACCEPTANCE OF NUCLEAR POWER GENERATION IN JAPAN AND RELEVANT INFLUENCING FACTORS

COMMUNITY ACCEPTANCE OF NUCLEAR POWER GENERATION IN JAPAN AND RELEVANT INFLUENCING FACTORS International Journal of Renewable Energy Research 1 (2011) 55-60 COMMUNITY ACCEPTANCE OF NUCLEAR POWER GENERATION IN JAPAN AND RELEVANT INFLUENCING FACTORS J. Cravioto, M. Bakr, S. Aoyagi, S. Park and

More information

Adequacy of Model Fit in Confirmatory Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Models: It Depends on What Software You Use

Adequacy of Model Fit in Confirmatory Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Models: It Depends on What Software You Use Adequacy of Model Fit in Confirmatory Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Models: It Depends on What Software You Use Susan R. Hutchinson University of Northern Colorado Antonio Olmos University of

More information

STATE OF NEVADA AGENCY FOR NUCLEAR PROJECTS/ NUCLEAR WASTE PROJECT OFFICE

STATE OF NEVADA AGENCY FOR NUCLEAR PROJECTS/ NUCLEAR WASTE PROJECT OFFICE STATE OF NEVADA AGENCY FOR NUCLEAR PROJECTS/ NUCLEAR WASTE PROJECT OFFICE NWPO-SE-032-90 Risk-Induced Social Impacts: The Effects of the Proposed Nuclear Waste Repository on residents of the Las Vegas

More information

Exploring success factors for Taiwan s government electronic tendering system: behavioral perspectives from end users

Exploring success factors for Taiwan s government electronic tendering system: behavioral perspectives from end users Exploring success factors for Taiwan s government electronic tendering system: behavioral perspectives from end users Pin-Yu Chu,a Naiyi Hsiao,b,* Fung-Wu Lee,a and Chun-Wei Chena ainstitute of Public

More information

Only Nuclear Energy Can Save the Planet

Only Nuclear Energy Can Save the Planet https://www.wsj.com/articles/only-nuclear-energy-can-save-the-planet-11547225861 The Wall Street Journal Only Nuclear Energy Can Save the Planet Do the math on replacing fossil fuels: To move fast enough,

More information

INDUSTRIAL ECOLOGY AND GREEN DESIGN - Risk Management And Industrial Ecology - Anatoly Ivanovich Mouravykh, RISK MANAGEMENT AND INDUSTRIAL ECOLOGY

INDUSTRIAL ECOLOGY AND GREEN DESIGN - Risk Management And Industrial Ecology - Anatoly Ivanovich Mouravykh, RISK MANAGEMENT AND INDUSTRIAL ECOLOGY RISK MANAGEMENT AND INDUSTRIAL ECOLOGY Anatoly Ivanovich Mouravykh, Department of Ecology and Management of Natural Resources, Russian Academy of Public Administration, Russia Keywords: ecology, safety

More information

Public Opinion about Nuclear Energy Year 2017 Poll

Public Opinion about Nuclear Energy Year 2017 Poll ABSTRACT Public Opinion about Nuclear Energy Year Poll Radko Istenič, Igor Jenčič Institut "Jožef Stefan" Jamova cesta 39 1000 Ljubljana, Slovenia radko.istenic@ijs.si, igor.jencic@ijs.si Public information

More information

2001 CUSTOMER SATISFACTION RESEARCH TRACKING STUDY

2001 CUSTOMER SATISFACTION RESEARCH TRACKING STUDY 2001 CUSTOMER SATISFACTION RESEARCH TRACKING STUDY FINAL REPORT Prepared For: Newfoundland & Labrador Hydro Hydro Place, Columbus Drive P.O. Box 12400 St. John s, NF A1B 4K7 Prepared By: www.marketquest.ca

More information

Executive Summary. What we did. When. Where. Who

Executive Summary. What we did. When. Where. Who Executive Summary This research establishes baseline measures of community wellbeing, community resilience and adaptation, and expected future wellbeing in the context of a proposed coal seam gas (CSG)

More information

Management Science Letters

Management Science Letters Management Science Letters 4 (2014) 1003 1008 Contents lists available at GrowingScience Management Science Letters homepage: www.growingscience.com/msl Examining the effect of relationship marketing on

More information

An Empirical Investigation of Consumer Experience on Online Purchase Intention Bing-sheng YAN 1,a, Li-hua LI 2,b and Ke XU 3,c,*

An Empirical Investigation of Consumer Experience on Online Purchase Intention Bing-sheng YAN 1,a, Li-hua LI 2,b and Ke XU 3,c,* 2017 4th International Conference on Economics and Management (ICEM 2017) ISBN: 978-1-60595-467-7 An Empirical Investigation of Consumer Experience on Online Purchase Intention Bing-sheng YAN 1,a, Li-hua

More information

PUBLIC PERCEPTION ON HYDROGEN INFRASTRUCTURE IN JAPAN

PUBLIC PERCEPTION ON HYDROGEN INFRASTRUCTURE IN JAPAN PUBLIC PERCEPTION ON HYDROGEN INFRASTRUCTURE IN JAPAN Itaoka, K. 1 *, Saito, A. 2 and Sasaki, K. 1 3 4 5 1 International Institute for Carbon-Neutral Energy Research, Kyushu University, 744 Motooka, Nishi-ku,

More information

Nuclear Accident in Japan: NRC Early Protective Action Recommendations

Nuclear Accident in Japan: NRC Early Protective Action Recommendations Nuclear Accident in Japan: NRC Early Protective Action Recommendations National Radiological Emergency Preparedness Conference April 18, 2011 Patricia A. Milligan, CHP Senior Technical Advisor for Preparedness

More information

1. Introduction Method and instruments Main outcomes Demographic characteristics of surveyed users... 5

1. Introduction Method and instruments Main outcomes Demographic characteristics of surveyed users... 5 User Satisfaction Survey 2014 Banja Luka, December 2014 TABLE OF CONTENTS 1. Introduction... 3 2. Method and instruments... 3 3. Main outcomes... 4 4. Demographic characteristics of surveyed users... 5

More information

The Risk, Reality and Future of Nuclear Power. Gregory Jaczko April 17, 2015

The Risk, Reality and Future of Nuclear Power. Gregory Jaczko April 17, 2015 The Risk, Reality and Future of Nuclear Power Gregory Jaczko April 17, 2015 The Risk, Reality, and Future of Nuclear Power Overview Why Nuclear Power is Still Around Risk Qualitative Health Objectives

More information

Predictors of pro-environmental behaviour in 1993 and 2010 An international comparison. Janine Chapman

Predictors of pro-environmental behaviour in 1993 and 2010 An international comparison. Janine Chapman Predictors of pro-environmental behaviour in 1993 and 2010 An international comparison Janine Chapman December 2013 Published by the Centre for Work + Life University of South Australia http://www.unisa.edu.au/hawkeinstitute/cwl/default.asp

More information

NOVA SCOTIA POWER CUSTOMER ENERGY FORUM

NOVA SCOTIA POWER CUSTOMER ENERGY FORUM NOVA SCOTIA POWER CUSTOMER ENERGY FORUM SUMMARY OF RESULTS November 19-20, 2004 Prepared by The Public Decision Partnership: Will Guild, Ph.D. Ron Lehr Dennis Thomas, Ph.D. Table of Contents EXECUTIVE

More information

Consumer attitudes and perceptions on sustainability

Consumer attitudes and perceptions on sustainability Consumer attitudes and perceptions on sustainability June 2010 Contents Summary Page 2 Part I: Methodology 4 Part II: Findings Consumer attitudes towards environmental and ethical topics 5 Environmental

More information

2012 International Symposium on Safety Science and Technology Research on the relationship between safety leadership and safety climate in coalmines

2012 International Symposium on Safety Science and Technology Research on the relationship between safety leadership and safety climate in coalmines Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Procedia Engineering 45 (2012 ) 214 219 2012 International Symposium on Science and Technology Research on the relationship between safety leadership and safety

More information

Available online at ScienceDirect. Procedia Economics and Finance 27 ( 2015 )

Available online at  ScienceDirect. Procedia Economics and Finance 27 ( 2015 ) Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Procedia Economics and Finance 27 ( 2015 ) 567 573 22nd International Economic Conference IECS 2015 Economic Prospects in the Context of Growing

More information

in partnership with EMPLOYEE OUTLOOK EMPLOYEE VIEWS ON WORKING LIFE

in partnership with EMPLOYEE OUTLOOK EMPLOYEE VIEWS ON WORKING LIFE in partnership with EMPLOYEE OUTLOOK EMPLOYEE VIEWS ON WORKING LIFE 2015 The CIPD is the professional body for HR and people development. The not-for-profit organisation champions better work and working

More information

SURVEY ON SOCIAL ENTREPRENEURS

SURVEY ON SOCIAL ENTREPRENEURS SURVEY ON SOCIAL ENTREPRENEURS RESULTS FROM NETHERLANDS Fransje Langers & Roel During Wageningen University and Research, the INDEX Methodological framework PAGE 3 1. Introduction of personal characteristics

More information

Montessori Model United Nations. Distr.: Middle School Twelfth Session XX March Fourth Committee Special Political and Decolonization Committee

Montessori Model United Nations. Distr.: Middle School Twelfth Session XX March Fourth Committee Special Political and Decolonization Committee Background Montessori Model United Nations General Assembly Distr.: Middle School Twelfth Session XX March 2017 Original: English Fourth Committee Special Political and Decolonization Committee This group

More information

Eeekonomics: BEG/CEE UT Annual Meeting. Commentary: European Live Issues, Post recession Demand. European gas demand post Fukushima Outline

Eeekonomics: BEG/CEE UT Annual Meeting. Commentary: European Live Issues, Post recession Demand. European gas demand post Fukushima Outline Gas Research Programme ENERGY STUDIES Natural G Eeekonomics: BEG/CEE UT Annual Meeting Houston, 7 December 2011 Commentary: European Live Issues, Post recession Demand Dr Anouk Honore Senior Research Fellow

More information

WIND FARM OPINION POLL. Conducted 11th 13th May 2015

WIND FARM OPINION POLL. Conducted 11th 13th May 2015 WIND FARM OPINION POLL Conducted 11th 13th May 2015 Abstract An insight into opinions regarding renewable energy and the environment in the Bournemouth area, and the feasibility of the Navitus Bay Wind

More information

Perception and Annoyance Related to Environmental Impacts of Coastal Wind Farms in Japan

Perception and Annoyance Related to Environmental Impacts of Coastal Wind Farms in Japan Proceedings of the 33 rd Annual Conference of IAIA, 2013 Perception and Annoyance Related to Environmental Impacts of Coastal Wind Farms in Japan Shigeo Nishikizawa*, Tatsuaki Mitani and Takehiko Murayama

More information

NUCLEAR ENERGY AND THE GLOBAL URANIUM MARKET POST FUKUSHIMA

NUCLEAR ENERGY AND THE GLOBAL URANIUM MARKET POST FUKUSHIMA 6 June 2011 Dear Toro Energy shareholder, NUCLEAR ENERGY AND THE GLOBAL URANIUM MARKET POST FUKUSHIMA I write to you to provide an update to Toro Energy s view of the impact on the nuclear power industry

More information

The Penalty of Part-time Work

The Penalty of Part-time Work Competence Centre for Diversity & Inclusion (CCDI) The Penalty of Part-time Work Effects and consequences of different working models, especially part-time work, for employers and employees in companies

More information

AN ANALYSIS OF FUNCTIONAL / PSYCHOLOGICAL BARRIERS AND QUALITY OF E-BANKING / INTERNET BANKING

AN ANALYSIS OF FUNCTIONAL / PSYCHOLOGICAL BARRIERS AND QUALITY OF E-BANKING / INTERNET BANKING CHAPTER 5 AN ANALYSIS OF FUNCTIONAL / PSYCHOLOGICAL BARRIERS AND QUALITY OF E-BANKING / INTERNET BANKING 5.1 Introduction India is one of the leading countries in the world where e-banking / internet banking

More information

OPPORTUNITIES FOR CAREER ADVANCEMENT FOUND TO BE VERY IMPORTANT TO TRUCKLOAD DRIVERS. Julene M. Rodriguez Gene C. Griffin

OPPORTUNITIES FOR CAREER ADVANCEMENT FOUND TO BE VERY IMPORTANT TO TRUCKLOAD DRIVERS. Julene M. Rodriguez Gene C. Griffin OPPORTUNITIES FOR CAREER ADVANCEMENT FOUND TO BE VERY IMPORTANT TO TRUCKLOAD DRIVERS By Julene M. Rodriguez Gene C. Griffin UGPTI Staff Paper No. 108 February 1993 OPPORTUNITIES FOR CAREER ADVANCEMENT

More information

VEYS OF CONSUME IVE Surveys of Consumers I TY OF MIC

VEYS OF CONSUME IVE Surveys of Consumers I TY OF MIC SURVEYS OF CONSUMERS UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN Surveys of Consumers The Surveys of Consumers are conducted by the Survey Research Center, under the direction of Richard T. Curtin, at the University of Michigan.

More information

Atoms for the Future 2014 Paris September National Debate and Public Confidence in Sweden

Atoms for the Future 2014 Paris September National Debate and Public Confidence in Sweden Atoms for the Future 2014 Paris 12-13 September National Debate and Public Confidence in Sweden Ted Lindquist Coordinator Association of Swedish Municipalities with Nuclear Facilities KSO Association of

More information

Journal of Radiation Research and Applied Sciences 8 (2015) 493e497. Available online at ScienceDirect

Journal of Radiation Research and Applied Sciences 8 (2015) 493e497. Available online at   ScienceDirect Journal of Radiation Research and Applied Sciences 8 (2015) 493e497 HOSTED BY Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Journal of Radiation Research and Applied Sciences journal homepage:

More information

Running head: THE MEANING AND DOING OF MINDFULNESS

Running head: THE MEANING AND DOING OF MINDFULNESS Running head: THE MEANING AND DOING OF MINDFULNESS Supplementary Materials Fully latent SEM version of model 1 Supplementary Fig 1 outlines the direct effects for the fully latent equivalent to the path

More information

tyuiopasdfghjklzxcvbnmqwertyuiopas dfghjklzxcvbnmqwertyuiopasdfghjklzx cvbnmqwertyuiopasdfghjklzxcvbnmq wertyuiopas

tyuiopasdfghjklzxcvbnmqwertyuiopas dfghjklzxcvbnmqwertyuiopasdfghjklzx cvbnmqwertyuiopasdfghjklzxcvbnmq wertyuiopas qwertyuiopasdfghjklzxcvbnmqwertyui opasdfghjklzxcvbnmqwertyuiopasdfgh jklzxcvbnmqwertyuiopasdfghjklzxcvb nmqwertyuiopasdfghjklzxcvbnmqwer DIVISION OF AGRICULTURE AND NATURAL RESOURCES SYSTEM WIDE CULTURAL

More information

OSJMUN Disarmament Committee

OSJMUN Disarmament Committee OSJMUN Disarmament Committee Issue : Quantifying the Amount of Nuclear Waste and Finding Solutions to The Issue Forum: Disarmament Committee Issue: Quantifying the Amount of Nuclear Waste and Finding Solutions

More information

Beyond balanced growth: The effect of human capital on economic growth reconsidered

Beyond balanced growth: The effect of human capital on economic growth reconsidered Beyond balanced growth 11 PartA Beyond balanced growth: The effect of human capital on economic growth reconsidered Uwe Sunde and Thomas Vischer Abstract: Human capital plays a central role in theoretical

More information

1 st ADDENDUM to the COMMENTS on the

1 st ADDENDUM to the COMMENTS on the Greenpeace in Zentral- und Osteuropa Fernkorngasse 10 t: +43 (1) 5454580 0 1100 Wien, Österreich i: www.greenpeace.at Correspondence address: Jan Haverkamp ul. Warynskiego 37A/10 PL 80-433 Gdansk Poland

More information

Lessons from Fukushima

Lessons from Fukushima Connecting the Dots on Energy Issues May 16, 2011 Vol. 4 Issue 6 1111 19 th St. NW Suite 406 Washington, DC 20036 tel: 202-461-2360 fax: 202-461-2379 secureenergy.org Lessons from Fukushima SUMMARY Even

More information

Period 18: Consequences of Nuclear Energy Use

Period 18: Consequences of Nuclear Energy Use Name Section Period 18: Consequences of Nuclear Energy Use As you watch the videos in class today, look for a pro-nuclear or anti-nuclear bias on the part of the video producers, narrators, and interviewers.

More information

Nanotechnologies from the consumers point of view

Nanotechnologies from the consumers point of view Nanotechnologies from the consumers point of view What consumers know and what they would like to know Antje Grobe, Mikko Rissanen, Philippe Funda, Joel De Beer, Uschi Jonas Edited by the Federal Office

More information

REPORT ON THE EUROSTAT 2016 USER SATISFACTION SURVEY

REPORT ON THE EUROSTAT 2016 USER SATISFACTION SURVEY EUROPEAN COMMISSION EUROSTAT Directorate A: Cooperation in the European Statistical System; international cooperation; resources Task Force Integrated Planning REPORT ON THE EUROSTAT 2016 USER SATISFACTION

More information

The Adaption Test: The Development of a Method to Measure Speed Adaption to Traffic Complexity

The Adaption Test: The Development of a Method to Measure Speed Adaption to Traffic Complexity University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2009 Driving Assessment Conference Jun 24th, 12:00 AM The Adaption Test: The Development of a Method to Measure Speed Adaption to Traffic

More information

Survey of Cohort Mentors: Gender-Based Analyses August 2011

Survey of Cohort Mentors: Gender-Based Analyses August 2011 Survey of Cohort Mentors: Gender-Based Analyses August 2011 1 Sample Nineteen mentors completed the survey from an overall population sample of 38 mentors. Thus, this survey has a response rate of 50.0%.

More information

Sustainable evaluation of urban buildings for safety in environment management: the case of the Taiwan 921 earthquake

Sustainable evaluation of urban buildings for safety in environment management: the case of the Taiwan 921 earthquake Sustainable Development and Planning IV, Vol. 2 537 Sustainable evaluation of urban buildings for safety in environment management: the case of the Taiwan 921 earthquake C.-H. Tu & K.-W. Tsou Department

More information

II.-1. Major nuclear power facilities in Japan

II.-1. Major nuclear power facilities in Japan II.-1. Major nuclear power facilities in Japan In Japan, there are 50 operable nuclear reactors in nuclear power plants in 17 locations and 6 reactors which are under decommissioning. In addition to these

More information

Activity C: The Chernobyl Disaster

Activity C: The Chernobyl Disaster The Chernobyl Disaster Teacher s Briefing Activity C: The Chernobyl Disaster Further notes Plenary activity Curriculum links Materials for Students Question sheet Map cards A3 map Download this resource

More information

Getting the Bead of Hardship

Getting the Bead of Hardship Getting the Bead of Hardship Benjamin Messer, Jordan Folks, and Jane Peters, Research Into Action, Portland, OR Alex Dunn, Portland, OR Prapti Gautam, Southern California Edison, Rosemead, CA ABSTRACT

More information

Effects of Civil Service Management Practices in Malawi Carolyne Barker and Brigitte Seim

Effects of Civil Service Management Practices in Malawi Carolyne Barker and Brigitte Seim Effects of Civil Service Management Practices in Malawi Carolyne Barker and Brigitte Seim The Survey of Public Servants The survey was executed in person among bureaucratic officials in Malawi between

More information

2016 EMPLOYEE SURVEY RESULTS AND ANALYSIS

2016 EMPLOYEE SURVEY RESULTS AND ANALYSIS 2016 EMPLOYEE SURVEY RESULTS AND ANALYSIS JULY 2016 Survey Administered by the Institutional Effectiveness Committee March-June 2016 Report Prepared by the Office of Institutional Advancement Data Support

More information

DECISION SCIENCES INSTITUTE An Analysis of the Effects of Global Sourcing Practices on Ethical Consumption in the United States. Full Paper Submission

DECISION SCIENCES INSTITUTE An Analysis of the Effects of Global Sourcing Practices on Ethical Consumption in the United States. Full Paper Submission DECISION SCIENCES INSTITUTE An Analysis of the Effects of Global Sourcing Practices on Ethical Consumption in the United States Full Paper Submission Robert L. Bregman University of Houston RBregman@uh.edu

More information

American Climate Metrics Survey New York City

American Climate Metrics Survey New York City American Climate Metrics Survey 2018 New York City BELIEF AND AWARENESS New Yorkers believe humans have a role in climate change and are personally concerned. American Climate Metrics Survey 2018 New York

More information

An Investigation of Contingency Factors Influencing Intellectual Capital Information

An Investigation of Contingency Factors Influencing Intellectual Capital Information An Investigation of Contingency Factors Influencing Intellectual Capital Information Mohammad Soheily Department of Accounting, Yazd Branch, Islamic Azad University, Yazd, Iran Safaieeh, Shoahadegomnam

More information

Background and Current Situation of Restricted Areas and Areas to Which Evacuation Orders Have Been Issued

Background and Current Situation of Restricted Areas and Areas to Which Evacuation Orders Have Been Issued Basic Concept and Issues to be Challenged for Rearranging the Restricted Areas and Areas to which Evacuation Orders Have been Issued where Step 2 has been Completed December 26, 2011 Nuclear Emergency

More information

Assuring the human resources for severe accident management and environmental remediation. Ian McKinley & Susie Hardie

Assuring the human resources for severe accident management and environmental remediation. Ian McKinley & Susie Hardie Assuring the human resources for severe accident management and environmental remediation Ian McKinley & Susie Hardie Introduction By their very nature, severe accidents tend to be unique in terms of their

More information

Chapter 11 Three Mile Island and Fukushima. Some Reflections on the History of Nuclear Power

Chapter 11 Three Mile Island and Fukushima. Some Reflections on the History of Nuclear Power Chapter 11 Three Mile Island and Fukushima Some Reflections on the History of Nuclear Power J. Samuel Walker Abstract This article draws comparisons between the Three Mile Island accident in 1979 and the

More information

Online Word of Mouth as a Determination in Adolescents Purchase Decision Making: the Influence of Expertise and Involvement

Online Word of Mouth as a Determination in Adolescents Purchase Decision Making: the Influence of Expertise and Involvement 1 Online Word of Mouth as a Determination in Adolescents Purchase Decision Making: the Influence of Expertise and Involvement Chih-Chien Wang, Graduate Institute of Information Management, National Taipei

More information

Safety Requirements for HTR Process Heat Applications

Safety Requirements for HTR Process Heat Applications HTR 2014 Conference, Oct. 2014, Weihai, China Norbert Kohtz (TÜV Rheinland), Michael A. Fütterer (Joint Research Centre): Safety Requirements for HTR Process Heat Applications Outline 1. Introduction 2.

More information

Survey of Cohort Mentors August 2011

Survey of Cohort Mentors August 2011 Survey of Cohort Mentors August 2011 1 Sample Nineteen mentors completed the survey from an overall population sample of 38 mentors. Thus, this survey has a response rate of 50%. This sample was equally

More information

Who Are My Best Customers?

Who Are My Best Customers? Technical report Who Are My Best Customers? Using SPSS to get greater value from your customer database Table of contents Introduction..............................................................2 Exploring

More information

QUANTITATIVE COMPARABILITY STUDY of the ICC INDEX and THE QUALITY OF LIFE DATA

QUANTITATIVE COMPARABILITY STUDY of the ICC INDEX and THE QUALITY OF LIFE DATA QUANTITATIVE COMPARABILITY STUDY of the ICC INDEX and THE QUALITY OF LIFE DATA Dr. Kseniya Rubicondo - November 2016 Table of Contents Introduction...p.3 Methodology. p.4 Analysis and Key Findings. p.5

More information

Nuclear plant?

Nuclear plant? What does Safety Culture Look like in a Nuclear plant? Helen Rycraft h.rycraft@iaea.org What is the IAEA? Created 1957 President Eisenhower Atoms for peace address to UN1953 Vienna HQ, Toronto, Tokyo,

More information

Impact of Fukushima Dai-ichi nuclear accident on global and Japan s nuclear energy policy

Impact of Fukushima Dai-ichi nuclear accident on global and Japan s nuclear energy policy Impact of Fukushima Dai-ichi nuclear accident on global and Japan s nuclear energy policy The University of Tokyo Workshop What Fukushima nuclear disaster brought about in Asia? - Emerging risks in social,

More information

Motivation, satisfaction, and retention

Motivation, satisfaction, and retention Motivation, satisfaction, and retention Understanding the importance of vets day-to-day work experiences Report by Dr Christopher Begeny, Professor Michelle Ryan and Dr Renata Bongiorno Psychology, University

More information

PRO/CON: The world's getting warmer, can nuclear power help us?

PRO/CON: The world's getting warmer, can nuclear power help us? PRO/CON: The world's getting warmer, can nuclear power help us? By Tribune News Service, adapted by Newsela staff on 02.04.16 Word Count 1,520 Dry cask storage units of nuclear fuel are pictured at the

More information

Sustainability and Legacy of Strategy Projects

Sustainability and Legacy of Strategy Projects Sustainability and Legacy of Strategy Projects Evaluation of the Stronger Families and Communities Strategy 2000-2004 Written by Associate Professor Patricia Rogers Ms Sue Kimberley Associate Professor

More information

Nuclear Philippines is a future full of

Nuclear Philippines is a future full of opinion.inquirer.net Nuclear Philippines is a future full of costly risks Laurence Delina 12:07 AM September 12th, 2016 CAMBRIDGE, Massachusetts The Duterte administration recently floated the revival

More information

Knowledge of Security Protocols and Acceptance of E-commerce

Knowledge of Security Protocols and Acceptance of E-commerce Knowledge of Security Protocols and Acceptance of E-commerce Sumendra Singh, Faculty of Business and IT, Webster University, Thailand. E-mail: singh89sumendra@gmail.com Leslie Klieb, Faculty of Business

More information

Reflections on Fukushima

Reflections on Fukushima Reflections on Fukushima 19 th International Conference On Nuclear Engineering (ICONE19) Osaka, Japan Dr. Nils J. Diaz Managing Director, The ND2 Group, LLC Chairman, ASME Task Force- Japan Events October

More information

Customer satisfaction as a gain/loss situation: Are experienced customers more loss aversive?

Customer satisfaction as a gain/loss situation: Are experienced customers more loss aversive? Customer satisfaction as a gain/loss situation: Are experienced customers more loss aversive? 1 Magnus Söderlund Center for Consumer Marketing, Stockholm School of Economics, P.O. Box 6501, SE-113 83 Stockholm,

More information

Complex modeling of factors influencing market success of new product and service developments

Complex modeling of factors influencing market success of new product and service developments Complex modeling of factors influencing market success of new product and service developments Identification of drivers of new product success and analysis of their relations are very critical for companies

More information

Evaluation of Operating Results for Japan Atomic Energy Agency in FY2015

Evaluation of Operating Results for Japan Atomic Energy Agency in FY2015 Evaluation of Operating Results for Japan Atomic Energy Agency in FY2015 September 2016 Minister of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology Minister of Economy, Trade and Industry Nuclear Regulation

More information

Rural and Urban Manufacturing Workers: Similar Problems, Similar Challenges

Rural and Urban Manufacturing Workers: Similar Problems, Similar Challenges United States Department of Agriculture Economic Research Service Agriculture Information Bulletin Number 736-02 January 1998 Rural and Urban Manufacturing Workers: Similar Problems, Similar Challenges

More information

Attitudes of Nebraska Residents on Nebraska Water Management

Attitudes of Nebraska Residents on Nebraska Water Management TECHNICAL REPORT 13-020 Attitudes of Nebraska Residents on Nebraska Water Management April 4, 2013 Submitted by Michelle L. Edwards Washington State University Don A. Dillman Washington State University

More information

EXAMINING THE RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN DEMAND CHAIN INTEGRATION, FLEXIBILITY, AND CUSTOMER OUTCOMES IN A B2B CONTEXT

EXAMINING THE RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN DEMAND CHAIN INTEGRATION, FLEXIBILITY, AND CUSTOMER OUTCOMES IN A B2B CONTEXT EXAMINING THE RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN DEMAND CHAIN INTEGRATION, FLEXIBILITY, AND CUSTOMER OUTCOMES IN A B2B CONTEXT Nalini Govindarajulu Assistant Professor of Operations Management C reighton University

More information