Sex differences on the WISC-R in Belgium and The Netherlands

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Sex differences on the WISC-R in Belgium and The Netherlands"

Transcription

1 Available online at Intelligence 36 (2008) Sex differences on the WISC-R in Belgium and The Netherlands Sophie van der Sluis a,, Catherine Derom b, Evert Thiery c, Meike Bartels a, Tinca J.C. Polderman a,d, F.C. Verhulst d, Nele Jacobs c, Sofie van Gestel c, Eco J.C. de Geus a, Conor V. Dolan e, Dorret I. Boomsma a, Danielle Posthuma a a Department of Biological Psychology, VU University Amsterdam, Van der Boechorststraat 1, 1081 BT Amsterdam, The Netherlands b Department of Human Genetics, University Hospital Gasthuisberg, Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, B-3000 Leuven, Belgium c Association for Scientific Research in Multiple Births, B-9070 Destelbergen, Belgium d Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Erasmus MC- Sophia, Dr. Molewaterplein 60, 3015 GJ, Rotterdam, The Netherlands e Department of Psychology, FMG, University of Amsterdam, Roeterstraat 15, 1018 WB, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Received 6 May 2006; received in revised form 15 January 2007; accepted 17 January 2007 Available online 22 February 2007 Abstract Sex differences on the Dutch WISC-R were examined in Dutch children (350 boys, 387 girls, age years) and Belgian children (370 boys, 391 girls, age years). Multi-group covariance and means structure analysis was used to establish whether the WISC-R was measurement invariant across sex, and whether sex differences on the level of the subtests were indicative of sex differences in general intelligence (g). In both samples, girls outperformed boys on the subtest Coding, while boys outperformed girls on the subtests Information and Arithmetic. The sex differences in the means of these three subtests could not be accounted for by the first-order factors Verbal, Performance, and Memory. Measurement invariance with respect to sex was however established for the remaining 9 subtest. Based on these subtests, no significant sex differences were observed in the means of the first-order factors, or the second-order g-factor. In conclusion, the cognitive differences between boys and girls concern subtest-specific abilities, and these sizeable differences are not attributable to differences in first-order factors, or the second-order factor g Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Intelligence; Sex-differences; Multi-group covariance and mean structure analysis; Measurement invariance 1. Introduction Sex differences on the WISC-R have been studied in the WISC-R standardization samples of the USA, Scotland, The Netherlands, and China, and in data from Mauritius, New Zealand, and Belgium (e.g., Born & Lynn, 1994; Dai & Lynn, 1994; Grégoire, 2000; Jensen & Reynolds, 1983; Lynn & Mulhern, 1991; Corresponding author. address: s.van.der.sluis@psy.vu.nl (S. van der Sluis). Lynn, Riane, Venables, Mednick, & Irwing, 2005). The results are largely comparable across countries. Consistently, large differences favoring girls are reported regarding the subtest Coding (effect sizes about.5), and large differences favoring boys are reported regarding the subtest Information (effect sizes about.35). In addition, girls sometimes outperform boys on the subtest Digit Span, but these differences are usually small and statistically insignificant. Boys score slightly higher than girls on all other subtests, and even though these differences are sometimes statistically significant, /$ - see front matter 2007 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi: /j.intell

2 S. van der Sluis et al. / Intelligence 36 (2008) the differences are often small, with effect sizes ranging between.00 and.20. In all these studies, WISC-R subtest scores and factor scores have been compared directly between boys and girls. Yet it has never been established whether the factor structure of the WISC-R is actually comparable or measurement invariant across sex (see below). The interpretation of group differences in subtest- or factors scores may be complicated greatly if the underlying factor structure differs between the groups. That is, if a test battery does not measure the same construct(s) in different groups, then group differences in test scores representing first or higher order factors are difficult to interpret. The aim of the present study is to find out whether the WISC-R is measurement invariant across sex in children before comparing subtest and factor scores between boys and girls. The factor structure underlying the WISC-R has been studied in clinical and non-clinical samples (e.g., Anderson & Dixon, 1995; Burton et al., 2001; Donders, 1993; Huberty, 1987; Kush et al., 2001; Meesters, van Gastel, Ghys, & Merckelbach, 1998; Wright & Dappen, 1982). Principal component analyses (PCA, e.g., Born & Lynn, 1994; Lynn & Mulhern, 1991; Rushton & Jensen, 2003), exploratory factor analyses (EFA, e.g., Dolan, 2000; Dolan & Hamaker, 2001; Kush et al., 2001), and confirmatory factor analyses (CFA, e.g., Burton et al., 2001; Dolan, 2000; Dolan & Hamaker, 2001; Keith, 1997; Kush et al., 2001; Oh, Glutting, Watkins, Youngstrom, & McDermott, 2004) have yielded either a two factor ( Verbal and Performance ), or a three factor solution ( Verbal, Performance, and Memory, also known as Freedom from distractibility ). In these models, general intelligence ( g ) was either operationalized as the first principal component (PCA), or as a second-order factor (CFA). Given the assumption that these latent factors underlie the performance on the level of the subtests, one question of interest is whether the observed sex differences at the level of the subtests are a function of differences in g,or of differences on the level of the broad primary factors of intelligence (e.g., Verbal intelligence, Performance intelligence and Memory). However, it may also be the case that the subtest differences are not attributable to common factor differences, but rather are a manifestation of differences in the specific ability that the subtest taps. If boys and girls differ with respect to the mean on a given subtest, and this difference cannot be explained by the mean differences on the latent factor, which is supposed to underlie performance on the subtest, then the subtest may be viewed as biased with respect to sex. The term bias does not imply that the observed mean difference is not real, rather the term, as used here, implies that the mean difference on the subtest is greater or smaller than that expected on the basis of the latent factor mean difference. According to this definition, the term bias refers to the subtest as an indicator of the common factor, which the subtest is supposed to measure. For example, it has been established that the Information subtest of the WAIS is biased with respect to sex. Specifically, the male advantage on this subtest, which is supposed to measure general knowledge, is too large to be accounted for by the common factor Verbal Comprehension (e.g., Dolan et al., 2006; Van der Sluis et al., 2006). The difference is not indicative of a difference with respect to Verbal Comprehension. However, it may well be indicative of a true male advantage in general knowledge. Establishing the exact nature of an observed (subtest) mean difference is important in the light of theories, in which sex differences are attributed to latent mean differences (e.g., a difference in Verbal Comprehension, or a difference in g). In previous studies aimed at identifying the source(s) of the sex differences, PCA was mostly used to investigate sex differences on the factors underlying intelligence. Sex differences were evaluated by calculating weighted linear combination of the subtests means, where the subtests factor loadings served as weights (e.g., Born & Lynn, 1994; Jensen & Reynolds, 1983; Lynn, Fergusson, & Horwood, 2005; Lynn & Mulhern, 1991; Lynn, Riane, et al., 2005). The general finding of these studies is that boys score higher on the Verbal and Performance factors, while girls score higher on the Memory factor. With respect to general intelligence, operationalized as the first principal component, boys usually score higher than girls, but effect sizes are often small (about.10), and the difference is not always statistically significant. When expressed on the conventional IQ-scale with a mean of 100 and standard deviation of 15, these sex differences range from 1 to 6 IQ points (e.g., Lynn, Fergusson, et al., 2005; Lynn, Riane, et al., 2005). All these results are however based on samples with a broad age-range (6 16 years), and it remains to be seen whether the factor structure of the WISC-R, and the effects reported for the (factor) means, are stable across age. One obvious problem concerning this PCA-based method of studying sex differences is that sex differences on the level of the weighted means of the observed subtest scores may be due to one or just a few of many subtests. For example, boys may outperform girls on the Verbal factor only because they outperformed girls on the subtest Information, while their performance on the other verbal subtests may even be inferior. In that case, it

3 50 S. van der Sluis et al. / Intelligence 36 (2008) is more accurate to conclude that boys outperform girls with respect to a specific cognitive ability, i.e., general knowledge, rather than suggesting that boys have higher Verbal intelligence. Stated succinctly, this method does not explicitly address the structure of the observed mean differences. The use of PCA to study group differences is however characterized by several other disadvantages. Because PCA, in contrast to EFA or CFA, is based on a data transformation rather than an explicit statistical model, it does not generally include statistical testing or explicit model fitting. As a consequence, goodness of fit is not evaluated, i.e., the question of whether the model provides a reasonable description of the structure of the data is not addressed. In addition, an explicit statistical procedure to test whether the factor structure underlying the given test battery is comparable across groups is not conducted. The comparability of the factor structure across groups is however of utmost importance if one wishes to make meaningful comparisons between the subtest scores or factor scores of different groups. Furthermore, within the context of PCA, competing hypotheses are not compared statistically (e.g., are sex differences present on the level of the primary factors of intelligence or rather on the level of the observed subtests only?). Finally, PCA is not suited for modeling measurement error in the subtest scores, which is sure to exist. An alternative method for testing group differences within the context of factor models is multi-group covariance and means structure analysis (MG-CMSA; Sörbom, 1974; Little, 1997; Widaman & Reise, 1997). This method provides a comprehensive, model-based means to investigate the main sources of group difference. MG-CMSA allows one to evaluate and compare the fit of alternative models, which correspond with competing hypotheses. The advantages of MG- CMSA have been studied and discussed in detail, and MG-CMSA has repeatedly been shown to be superior to other methods used to study group difference (e.g., method of correlated vectors, the Schmidt Leiman procedure, PCA) with regard to, among things, its flexibility and the facility to test (competing) hypotheses explicitly (see e.g., e.g., Dolan, 2000; Dolan & Hamaker, 2001; Dolan, Roorda, & Wicherts, 2004; Lubke, Dolan, & Kelderman, 2001; Lubke, Dolan, Kelderman, & Mellenbergh, 2003; Millsap, 1997). Previously, MG-CMSA was used to study ethnic group difference in intelligence (e.g., Dolan, 2000; Dolan & Hamaker, 2001; Dolan et al., 2004; Gustafsson, 1992), the Flynn-effect (Wicherts et al., 2004), and sex differences on the WAIS (Dolan et al., 2006; Van der Sluis et al., 2006). In the present study, we used MG-CMSA to investigate sex differences on the Dutch WISC-R in Dutch and Belgian children of limited age-range (9 13 years old). Below, we outline the MG-CMSA modeling procedure that we used to investigate the sources of sex differences on the WISC-R. Both firstand second-order factor models are fitted, with the second-order factor representing g. The results are presented for Dutch and Belgian subjects separately, and are discussed in the light of previous studies. 2. Method 2.1. Subjects Dutch sample The Dutch data constitute a combination of two datasets: data that were previously used in a study of the genetic and environmental contributions to the development of individual differences in intelligence (Bartels, Rietveld, van Baal, & Boomsma, 2002), and data that were used to establish the extent to which the phenotypic correlation between working memory speed and capacity is of genetic origin (Polderman et al., 2006). All Dutch subjects were recruited from the young Netherlands Twin Register (NTR, Boomsma, 1998; Boomsma et al., 2002; Bartels, Beijsterveldt, Stroet, Hudziak, & Boomsma, in press). Since 1986, the majority of parents with multiple births in The Netherlands receive a brochure and a registration form from the NTR. Registration is voluntary, and about 40% of the parents register their twins with the NTR. Information from questionnaires, blood group, and DNA polymorphisms (genetic markers) was used to assign zygosity to same-sex twins (Rietveld et al., 2000). For this study, data were available from 368 twin pairs (77 monozygotic male pairs, 100 monozygotic female pairs, 67 dizygotic male pairs, 62 dizygotic female pairs, and 62 opposite sex twins), and, due to missingness, one single twin. As in most of the twin studies, the percentage of MZ twins in this sample (48%) is somewhat higher than in the overall population ( 33%) due to self-selection bias. The sample included 350 boys and 387 girls (737 subjects in total). For all twins in this study, level of parental occupation was assessed at age 10 of the twins. Occupational level was rated on a 5-point scale, ranging from manual labor to academic employment. Paternal occupational level was used, or maternal occupational level in case paternal information was not available. In comparison to the Dutch population (Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek, 2002), the level of occupation of the

4 S. van der Sluis et al. / Intelligence 36 (2008) parents of the twins participating in this study was somewhat higher: the percentages observed in the present study and the Dutch population are: 1% and 6% (manual), 15% and 26% (lower), 42% and 40% (middle), 30% and 19% (higher), and 11% and 9% (academic). Paternal and maternal educational level did not differ between the boys and girls in this sample (z=.19, ns, and z=.20, ns, respectively). With the exception of one twin pair aged 10.9 years old, the age of all subjects ranged between 11.9 and 12.9 years at the time of testing. The youngest twin pair at 10.9 years old was not removed from the sample as it did not constitute an outlier in any aspect. Sex differences with respect to age were absent (t(735) b 1, ns) Belgian sample The Belgian subjects were recruited from the East Flanders Prospective Twins Survey (EFPTS), a population-based register of twins in the province of East Flanders, Belgium (Derom et al., 2002; Loos, Derom, Vlietinck, & Derom, 1998). Since 1964, EFPTS collects information on the mother, the placenta and the child of 98% of all multiples born in the province. Zygosity of all twins was determined through sequential analysis based on sex, foetal membranes, umbilical cord blood groups (ABO, Rh, CcDEe, Mnss, Duffy, Kell), placental alkaline phosphatase and, since 1982, DNA fingerprints. Unlike-sex twins and same-sex twins with at least one different genetic marker were classified as DZ; monochorionic twins were classified as MZ. For all same-sex dichorionic twins with the same genetic markers a probability of monozygosity was calculated. All subjects, whose data are used in the present study, participated in an ongoing study on cognitive ability in twins aged 7.5 to 15 years old. This sample was shown to be representative for gender, birth weight, and gestational age. As in most of the twin studies, the MZ twins were slightly over represented (42%) due to self-selection biases. Comparison of the 663 twins with known IQ scores with the twins who refused to participate in the study (n=204) revealed that, in the non-participating group, parents with a lower educational level and twins who attend special schools tend to be over represented. Part of the data (only complete same-sexed twin pairs with known IQ scores) was previously used to study the effect of chorion-type on the estimation of the heritability of intelligence (Jacobs et al., 2001). The present dataset comprises a subsample (agerange between 9.5 and 13 years at time of measurement) of the above-mentioned study. The sample consisted of 370 boys and 391 girls (761 subjects in total). Data were available from 374 complete twin pairs (83 monozygotic male pairs, 76 monozygotic female pairs, 44 dizygotic male pairs, 63 dizygotic female pairs, and 108 opposite sex twin pairs), and 13 single twins. Sex differences with respect to age were absent (t(759) =1.73, ns) Tests In both Dutch and Belgian samples, psychometric IQ was measured with the following 12 subtests of the Dutch WISC-R (Van Haasen et al., 1986): Information (INF), Similarities (SIM), Arithmetic (AR), Vocabulary (VOC), Comprehension (COMP), Picture Completion (PC), Picture Arrangement (PA), Block Design (BD), Object Assembly (OA), Mazes (MA), Coding (CO), and Digit span (DS). In the Belgian data missingness was absent. The Dutch data, in contrast, included systematic missing data. Specifically, for reasons of efficiency, only 6 out of 12 subtests (namely SIM, AR, VOC, BD, OA, and DS) were administered to the 354 Dutch subjects (165 boys, 189 girls) who previously participated in the study by Polderman et al. (2006). Some additional missingness occurred due to procedural errors, but this percentage was very small (.2%). Due to this systematic omission of subtests in part of the Dutch sample, missingness can not be considered completely at random (MCAR, e.g., Schafer & Graham, 2002) in the total Dutch sample (p b.01 for Little's MCAR test performed across families and for boys and girls separately). In the following exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses, raw data Maximum Likelihood estimation was employed to accommodate the missingness, and use all available data. Raw data ML estimation has been found to provide better parameter estimates than conventional methods, such as listwise or pairwise deletion and mean imputation, even if data are not missing (completely) at random (e.g., Tomarken & Waller, 2005) Statistical analyses Measurement invariance and model fitting strategies To study sex differences in the means and covariances within the common factor model, multi-group confirmatory covariance and means structure analysis (MG- CMSA) was used. Before sex differences with respect to the latent common factors can be examined, we first need to establish whether the WISC-R is measurement invariant with respect to sex. Measurement invariance with regard to sex implies that the distribution of the observed scores on a subtest (y i ) given a fixed level of the

5 52 S. van der Sluis et al. / Intelligence 36 (2008) latent factor (η), depends on the score on the latent factor η, and not sex, i.e., f [ y i η,sex]=f [ y i η] (Mellenbergh, 1989). Given normally distributed data, measurement invariance can be defined in terms of the means and the variances of the y i given η. With respect to the means, measurement invariance implies that the expected value of subjects i on subtest y depends only on the latent factor score η, and not on sex, i.e., E[y i η,sex]=e[y i η]. Within the common factor model, to establish measurement invariance one needs to establish whether the relation between the observed subtest scores and the underlying latent factors is the same in boys and girls (Meredith, 1993). Measurement invariance can be established through the imposition of a series of specific constraints on the model parameters over groups, i.e., across sex in this case (Meredith, 1993). First of all, the subtests should load on the same factors in both boys and girls, i.e., the measurement model should be the same in both sexes (also called configural invariance). Subsequently, the function relating the observed subtest scores to the latent factors can be considered identical for boys and girls if the following parameters can, to reasonable approximation, be considered equal across sex: a) the factor loadings of the observed subtests on the latent factors, b) the intercepts (note that the factor means are allowed to differ across groups), and c) the residual variances, i.e., the variance in the observed subtest scores that is not explained by the latent common factors. If these constraints prove tenable, the WISC-R may be considered to be measurement invariant with respect to sex, and in that case, individual differences and group differences on the level of the observed subtests can be interpreted in terms of differences on the common factors. The model fitting strategy that follows from the above described equality constraints is described in detail in Van der Sluis et al. (2006). Below we give an overview of this model fitting procedure, and we refer to Appendix A of Van der Sluis et al. (2006) for a description of this procedure in matrix notation First-order factor models First, we fitted the least constrained model, model F 1, that tests for configural invariance (Horn & McArdle, 1992; Widaman & Reise, 1997). Configural invariance implies that the configuration of factor loadings (and correlated residuals, if any) is the same across sex, but the exact values of these parameters are allowed to differ across groups. In this model, the observed means of the 12 subtests are estimated freely in boys and girls, i.e., we do not yet introduce a constrained model for the mean structure. Note that in model F 1, we fixed the variances of the latent factors to 1 in both boys and girls. This is a standard identifying constraint in factor analysis (e.g., see Bollen, 1989). Subsequently, we tested for metric invariance (Horn & McArdle, 1992; Widaman & Reise, 1997) by constraining the factor loadings to be identical across sex. We denote this model F 2. Identical factor loadings are a prerequisite for a meaningful comparison between boys and girls with respect to the latent common factors, i.e., if the factor loadings of the subtests on the latent factors are not identical across sex, we cannot be sure that the latent factors are identical, and thus comparable, across sex. If the constraints introduced in model F 2 do not result in a significant deterioration of the model fit compared to model F 1, metric invariance is considered tenable. Note that these equality constraints on the factor loadings render fixation of the factorial variance in both group superfluous, so in model F 2, the factor variances remain fixed to 1 in the boys, but are estimated freely in the girls. Next, we test for strong factorial invariance (Horn & McArdle, 1992; Meredith, 1993; Widaman & Reise, 1997) by introducing a restrictive structure for the means. We denote this model F 3. In model F 3, the intercepts in the regression of the observed variables on the common factors are constrained to be equal in boys and girls, while the means of the factors are estimated. We thus introduce a constrained model for the means structure. Note that for reasons of identification, it is not possible to estimate the factor means in both groups (Sörbom, 1974). We chose to fix the factor means to zero in the boys, and estimated freely in the girls. Modeled as such, the boys function as a reference group, and the factor means in the female group are calculated as deviations from the factor means of the boys. If the fit of model F 3 is not significantly worse than the fit of model F 2, the assumption that the expected values of the observed subtest scores depend not on sex but only on the latent factor scores, is considered tenable, i.e., E [ y i η,sex]=e[ y i η]. Model F 3 thus embodies the test whether the latent common factors can account for the observed mean differences between boys and girls on the level of the subtests. Boys and girls can be compared meaningfully with respect to their first-order factors means only if model F 3 holds. If model F 3 is not tenable, one or more of the mean differences between boys and girls on the level of the observed subtest scores cannot be accounted for by the first-order factors. As explained above, subtests are considered biased with respect to sex if observed difference on these tests cannot be attributed to differences on the level of the primary factors of intelligence. We next test for strict factorial invariance (Horn & McArdle, 1992;Meredith, 1993; Widaman & Reise, 1997) by constraining the residual variances to be equal across sex. We denote this model F 4. If model F 4 is

6 S. van der Sluis et al. / Intelligence 36 (2008) tenable, in comparison to model F 3, we conclude that all differences between boys and girls with respect to the means and the covariance structure can be accounted for by sex differences in the first-order factors. Note that the tenability of model F 4 is not a prerequisite for the comparability of boys and girls with respect to the observed means, or with respect to the means of the firstand second-order latent factors. To this end, F 3 suffices Second-order factors A second-order factor (model S 1 ), i.e., the model including general intelligence g as a second-order factor, was introduced in either model F 3 or F 4, depending on the tenability of the constraints introduced in model F 4. Depending on the number of first-order factors, this hierarchical factor model is either equivalent to the first-order factor model (in the case of 3 firstorder factors), or it tests whether all relations between the first-order factors can be explained by 1 secondorder factor (in the case of 4 or more first-order factors). At this point, the second-order factor loadings, i.e., the loadings of the first-order factors on the second-order factor, are allowed to differ across sex, and the means of the second-order factors are fixed to zero in both groups, while the first-order factor means are fixed to zero in boys, and freely estimated in girls (as in models F 3 and F 4 ). For reasons of identification, the variance of the second-order factor is fixed to 1 in both groups. In model S 2, the second-order factor loadings are constrained to be equal across sex. Like in model F 2, these equality constraints on the factor loadings allow one to freely estimate the variance of the second-order factor in one of the groups (in our case the girls). With model S 2 we thus test whether the factor loadings of the first-order factors on the second-order factor are equal in boys and girls. In model S 3, the first-order factor means are constrained to be equal across sex. Given our present parameterization, this involves fixing the first-order factor means differences to zero in the girls. The second-order factor mean is then fixed to zero in the boys, and estimated freely in the girls (analogous to model F 3 ). In this model, the mean difference between boys and girls are described entirely in terms of mean differences on the second-order factor, i.e., in g. IfmodelS 3 is tenable (in comparison to model S 2 ), we conclude that the mean differences between boys and girls on the level of the observed subtests can be accounted for completely by differences in g. If model S 3 does not fit the data, we conclude that the differences between boys and girls at the level of the first-order factors are not (or not completely) attributable to difference between boys and girls in g. In the final model, model S 4, we constrain the second-order factor means to be equal across sex (i.e., we fix the second-order factor mean difference to zero). If model S 4 fits as well as model S 3, we conclude that boys and female do not differ with respect to g. A significant deterioration of the fit as a result of this constraint indicates the presence of sex differences in g Estimation and model fit Both the Dutch and Belgian data were gathered within families. The focus of this paper, however, is on gender differences, and not on the correlations among family members, which are undoubtedly present as cognitive abilities are known to be quite heritable (e.g., Bartels et al., 2002; Daniels, Devlin, & Roeder, 1997; Posthuma et al., 2002). Treating within-family data as if they are independently distributed observations results in incorrect standard errors and incorrect χ 2 goodness of fit values, while the point estimations of parameter estimates remain unbiased (e.g., Rebollo, de Moor, Dolan & Boomsma, 2006). All factor analytic analyses were therefore performed in Mplus, version 4 (Muthén & Muthén, 2005), which computes corrected standard errors and Satorra Bentler scaled χ 2 -tests, taking into account the dependence of observations. Competing hypotheses, represented by different nested models (where the nested model is the more restricted model), can be compared through a weighted χ 2 -difference test developed especially for the comparison of the Satorra Bentler scaled χ 2 s(satorra, 2000). The more restricted model is accepted as the preferred model, if its fit is not significantly worse than the fit of the less restrictive model, i.e., if the χ 2 -difference test (henceforth the χ 2 diff ) is not significant. Below, we will not report scaled χ 2 - values for each model separately, as these are not 2 informative, rather we report weighted χ diff tests for the comparison between competing models. Given the large sample sizes, and the number of tests that were required to compare all ensuing models, we chose an α of.01. To evaluate the fit of the ensuing models to the data, the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the comparative fit index (CFI) were used (e.g., Bentler, 1990; Bollen & Long, 1993; Jöreskog, 1993; Schermelleh-Engel, Moosbrugger, & Müller, 2003). The RMSEA is a measure of the error of approximation of the covariance and mean structures as implied by the specified model to the covariance and mean structures in the population. As a measure of approximationdiscrepancy per degree of freedom, this fit index favors more parsimonious models. Generally, good fitting models are thought to have RMSEA b.05, although simulation studies by Hu and Bentler (1999) showed

7 54 S. van der Sluis et al. / Intelligence 36 (2008) that a cut-off criterion of.06 can be used as well. Here we adopt the following rule of thumb: a RSMEA of.05 or less indicates good approximation, RMSEA between.05 and.08 indicates reasonable approximation, and RMSEA greater than.08 indicates poor approximation (Browne & Cudeck, 1993; Schermelleh-Engel et al., 2003). The CFI is based on the comparison of the fit of the target model (i.e., the user-specified model) with the fit of the independence model (i.e., a model in which all variables are modeled as unrelated). Like the RMSEA, the CFI favors more parsimonious models. CFI ranges from zero to 1.00, and values N.90 or.95 are usually taken as indicative of adequate model fit (e.g., Hu & Bentler, 1999; Schermelleh-Engel et al., 2003). When testing for measurement invariance, the scaled χ 2 statistic was used to compare the fit of the competing models, while the RMSEA and the CFI were used only to check that the general fit of the ensuing models was still acceptable. In addition, modification indices were used to detect local misspecifications in the models. The modification index of a constrained parameter (i.e., fixed to a given value or subject to an equality constraint) expresses the expected drop in overall χ 2, if the constraint on the parameter is relaxed. 3. Results All analyses were performed on the standardized subtest scores, which have a mean of 10 and SD of 3 in the population Preliminary analyses Table 1 contains means and standard deviations of all 12 standardized subtest scores, reported separately for Dutch and Belgian boys and girls. As a measure of effect size, Cohen's d is also reported, which is calculated as the difference between the mean of the boys and the girls (μ boys μ girls ) divided by the pooled standard deviation, so that positive (negative) d's denote male (female) advantage. Most effect sizes were small (b.3 ), and medium effect sizes (between.3 and.6 ) were only observed with respect to INF and AR (favoring boys) and CO (favoring girls). It is possible that the differences between boys and girls are indicative of differences between families in, for example, socioeconomic status (SES), rather than of genuine sex differences. It is impossible to measure all variables on which families might differ, but comparing boys and girls who grew up in the same family environment provides a powerful check of the possible influence of family background. Opposite sex twin pairs are therefore of special interest. If the sex differences that are observed across families remain significant within families, then these differences are more likely to represent real differences between boys and girls. However, if these between family differences diminish, or even disappear, within families, the between family differences are more likely to relate to environmental differences. (Note that the opposite twin design does not imply perfect matching of boys and girls; e.g., Table 1 Means (M) and standard deviations (SD) for the Dutch and Belgian boys and girls on the 12 WISC-R subtests Netherlands Belgium Boys Girls Boys Girls M SD N M SD N d M SD N M SD N d INF SIM AR VOC COMP PC PA BD OA CO MA DS Scores are standardized scores (in norm sample, M=10, SD=3). Note. d is Cohen's measure of effect size d, defined as (M boys M girls /σ pooled ). INF =Information, SIM =Similarities, AR= Arithmetic, VOC= Vocabulary, COMP =Comprehension, PC =Picture Completion, PA =Picture Arrangement, BD=Block Design, OA=Object Assembly, MA=Mazes, CO=Coding, DS=Digit span.

8 S. van der Sluis et al. / Intelligence 36 (2008) systematic differences may exist in the way that boys and girls are treated). To more closely examine the mean differences, paired t-tests were performed on the data of the Dutch and Belgian opposite sex twin pairs (Table 2). In both the Dutch and Belgian samples, boys scored significantly higher on INF than their female sibling, while girls scored significantly higher on CO than their male sibling. These results are consistent with the effect sizes in Table 1, and likely to represent genuine differences between boys and girls. In addition, Dutch boys scored higher on AR, VOC and COMP than their female siblings. These latter results are consistent with the intermediate effect sizes for the Dutch sample as presented in Table 1. In sum, the results of the paired t-tests correspond to the differences observed between boys and girls in the total sample, and the sex differences are therefore not likely to be the result of between family differences in factors like SES. The mean differences between boys and girls on the level of the observed subtest scores, and the relation with the underlying primary factors of intelligence are further examined using MG-CMSA Exploratory factor analyses Because the reported patterns of factor loadings vary across studies, exploratory factor analyses (EFA) were carried out first to establish the pattern of factor loadings in Dutch and Belgian boys and girls separately. The Table 2 Paired t-tests for Dutch and Belgian opposite sex twins Netherlands Belgium (N pairs =62) (N pairs =108) t df p t df p INF SIM AR VOC COMP PC PA BD OA CO MA DS Note. Positive t-values indicate male advantage; negative t-values indicate female advantage. INF = Information, SIM = Similarities, AR = Arithmetic, VOC = Vocabulary, COMP = Comprehension, PC = Picture Completion, PA = Picture Arrangement, BD = Block Design, OA = Object Assembly, MA = Mazes, CO = Coding, DS = Digit span. Table 3 Results exploratory factor analyses, separately for Dutch and Belgian boys and girls Netherlands Boys Girls (N=350) (N=387) V P M V P M INF SIM AR VOC COM PC PA BP OA CO MA DS Belgium Boys Girls (N=370) (N=391) V P M V P M INF SIM AR VOC COMP PC PA BD OA CO MA DIG INF=Information, SIM=Similarities, AR=Arithmetic, VOC=Vocabulary, COMP=Comprehension, PC=Picture Completion, PA=Picture Arrangement, BD=Block Design, OA=Object Assembly, MA=Mazes, CO=Coding, DS=Digit span, V=Verbal factor, P=Performance factor, M=Memory factor. exploratory factor solution was followed by an oblique rotation (Promax; see Lawley & Maxwell, 1971), using normal theory maximum likelihood estimation (ML). In both Dutch and Belgian samples, solutions with one factor or with two correlated factors were inadequate, while the solution with three correlated factors proved reasonable in terms of goodness of fit and interpretability. Table 3 contains the loadings of the 12 subtests on the three correlated common factors reported separately for Dutch and Belgian boys and girls. Factor loadings in bold print were considered substantial in all subsamples. This empirically established pattern of factor loadings is largely similar to factor solutions

9 56 S. van der Sluis et al. / Intelligence 36 (2008) Table 4 Fit statistics for the Dutch models CFI RMSEA χ 2 diff F 1 Configural invariance F 1a Configural invariance+residuals OA and BD correlated F 1a vs. F 1 : χ 2 diff(2)=26.84, pb.001 F 2 Metric invariance F 2 vs. F 1a : χ 2 diff(11)=9.43, ns F 3 Strong factorial invariance F 3 vs. F 2 : χ 2 diff(9)=65.66, pb.001 F 3a Strong factorial invariance, bar INF, AR and CO F 3a vs. F 2 : χ 2 diff(6)=10.75, ns F 4 Strict factorial invariance F 4 vs. F 3a : χ 2 diff(13)=12.80, ns S 1 Introduction 2nd order factor S 1 is identical to F 4 S 2 Metric invariance 2nd order factor S 2 vs. S 1 : χ 2 diff(2)=5.34, ns S 3 Strong factorial invariance 2nd order factor S 3 vs. S 2 : χ 2 diff(2)=1.79, ns S 4 Strict factorial invariance 2nd order factor S 4 vs. S 3 : χ 2 diff(1)=4.20, ns (p=.04) reported in previous papers (e.g., Burton et al., 2001; Dolan, 2000; Dolan & Hamaker, 2001; Keith, 1997; Kush et al., 2001; Meesters et al., 1998; Oh et al., 2004), with the first factor representing the Verbal factor, the second factor the Performance factor, and the third factor the Memory factor (also known as Freedom from Distractibility: a mix of memory and speed). This configuration of factor loadings was subsequently used in the confirmatory MG-CMSA, with the bold factor loadings estimated freely, and all other factor loadings fixed to zero Confirmatory factor analyses Dutch sample The results and fit statistics of the MG-CMSA on the Dutch data are presented in Table First-order factor models. In model F 1 we tested for configural invariance: a factor model with three correlated factors was fitted in boys and girls separately, with INF, SIM, AR, VOC and COMP loading on the Verbal factor, OA, BD, PC and PA loading on the Performance factor, and AR, BD, CO, MA and DS on the Memory factor. All these factor loadings, which were estimated separately in the two sexes, were significant in both boys and girls. The modification indices (MIs) showed however that the fit of this baseline model could be improved substantially by allowing the residuals of OA and BD to correlate (MI=20 in boys, and MI=18 in girls). Note that such minor modifications of the Wechsler-model are not uncommon, and this specific link between OA and BD has been established before (e.g., Arnau & Thompson, 2000; Dolan et al., 2006; Ward, Axelrod, & Ryan, Fig. 1. First-order factor model for Dutch sample, where the λ's denote the regressions of the 12 subtests on the three factors, the Ψ 's denote the correlations between the factors, and the ε's denote those parts of the variances in the subtests that are not predicted by the factors, i.e., the residual variances. VERB=Verbal factor, MEM=Memory factor, PERF=Performance intelligence, INF=Information, SIM=Similarities, AR=Arithmetic, VOC=Vocabulary, COMP=Comprehension, PC=Picture Completion, PA=Picture Arrangement, BD=Block Design, OA=Object Assembly, MA=Mazes, CO=Coding, DS=Digit span.

10 S. van der Sluis et al. / Intelligence 36 (2008) ). Addition of these parameters to model F 1a in female and male samples resulted in a significant improvement of the fit (χ 2 diff (2)=26.84, p b.001). Model F 1a, which is depicted in Fig. 1, will serve as the baseline model for all subsequent analyses. As the configuration of factor loadings and correlated residuals was identical in boys and girls, configural invariance across sex was established in the Dutch sample. In model F 2 we tested for metric invariance by constraining the 14 factor loadings to be equal across sex, while the variances of the three common factors were freely estimated in the girls, and fixed to 1 in the boys for reasons of identification. These constraints did not result in a significant deterioration of the fit, compared to model F 1a (χ 2 diff (11)=9.43, ns), so the factor loadings can be considered identical across sex. Strong factorial invariance was tested in model F 3 by constraining the intercepts to be equal across sex, while the factorial means were fixed to zero in the boys, and estimated as latent mean differences in the girls. The fit of this model was however significantly worse than the fit of model F 2 (χ 2 diff (9)=65.66, pb.001), implying that not all mean sex differences on the level of the subtests can be accounted for by differences on the level of the first-order factors. In view of the MIs, in model F 3a, it was decided to constrain all intercepts to be equal across sex, except the intercepts of INF, AR, and CO. Model F 3a did not fit significantly worse than model F 2 (χ 2 diff (6)=10.75, ns). This means that strong factorial invariance was established for 9 of the 12 subtests. The sex differences on the subtests INF, AR and CO were too large to be accounted for by the first-order factors. So, in the sense discussed above, these three subtests may be viewed as biased within the common factor model. In all subsequent models, the subtest means of INF, AR, and CO were therefore estimated freely in each group, thereby effectively eliminating these subtests from the means model, while all other subtest means remained constrained to be equal across groups. Note that subtests that are biased with respect to their means can be retained in the model without consequence because, once these subtests' means have been relaxed (i.e., allowed to vary over sex), these indicators no longer contribute to the model for the means. Strict factorial invariance was tested in model F 4 by constraining the residual variances plus the correlated residuals to be equal across sex. The fit of model F 4 was not significantly worse than the fit of model F 3a (χ 2 diff (13)=12.80, ns), so the (correlated) residuals could be considered identical in boys and girls. The factor correlations and the factor means of this model are presented in Table 5. We find practically no sex Table 5 Correlations between the first-order factors Verbal, Performance, and Memory for Dutch boys (below diagonal) and girls (above diagonal), and the means and standard deviations for boys and girls on the firstorder factors Verbal Performance Memory Verbal Performance Memory Boys (N=350) Mean SD Girls (N=387) Mean SD Effect size Note. The means of the girls should be interpreted as deviations from the means of the boys, and were not significantly different from those of the boys (as tested in model S 3 ). difference with respect to Memory (.01, s.e.), and small differences with respect to Verbal (.26, s.e.) and Performance (.19, s.e.). Fixing the first-order factor mean differences to be equal across sex did not result in a significant deterioration of model fit (χ 2 diff (3)=6.49, p=.09), i.e., boys and girls did not differ significantly with respect to their means on the first-order factors. We note however, that the missingness present in the Dutch data may have reduced the statistical power to detect small factor mean differences between the sexes. In sum, full measurement invariance was not tenable as the sex differences on INF, AR and CO were too large to be accounted for by the first-order factors. Partial measurement invariance was however tenable for the remaining 9 subtests, and all small (and non-significant) sex differences as observed on the level of these subtests could be described as (non-significant) differences on the level of the first-order factors. Although no significant mean differences were observed between boys and girls on the level of the first-order factors, a more parsimonious model for the means may identify a significant effect for sex. We therefore proceed in studying sex differences with respect to the secondorder factor g Second-order factors models. In model S 1, g, was introduced as a second-order factor for general intelligence. However, as there were only three firstorder factors, this model with three first-order factors loading on 1 second-order factor was statistically equivalent to the model without a second-order factor, in which the first-order factors were simply correlated. The fit of model S 1 was thus identical to the fit of model F 4. Model S 1 is illustrated in Fig. 2.

11 58 S. van der Sluis et al. / Intelligence 36 (2008) Fig. 2. The hierarchical factor model, where the λ's denote the regressions of the 12 subtests on the three first-order factors, the γ's denote the regressions of the first-order factor on the second-order factor g, and the ζ's and ε's denote those parts of the variances in the subtests and first-order factors that are not predicted by the first-order factors and the second-order factor, respectively. Note that the model is identical for the Dutch and Belgian sample, except that in the Belgian sample age-effects were regressed out on the level of the subtests (not drawn here for convenience). g =factor for general intelligence, VERB=Verbal factor, MEM=Memory factor, PERF=Performance intelligence, INF=Information, SIM=Similarities, AR=Arithmetic, VOC=Vocabulary, COMP=Comprehension, PC=Picture Completion, PA=Picture Arrangement, BD=Block Design, OA=Object Assembly, MA=Mazes, CO=Coding, DS=Digit span. In model S 2, the second-order factor loadings were constrained to be equal across sex, and the variance of the second-order factor was fixed to 1 in the boys (for reasons of identification) and estimated freely in the girls. The fit of model S 2 was not significantly worse than the fit of model S 1 (χ 2 diff (2) =5.34, ns), i.e., the factor loadings of the three first-order factors on g are identical across sex. In model S 3, all first-order factor means were constrained to be zero in both boys and girls, while the mean of the second-order factor g was constrained to zero in boys for reasons of identification, and estimated freely in girls. The fit of the model did not deteriorate significantly as a result of these constraints (χ 2 diff (2)=.79, ns), meaning that the sex differences with respect to the means of the first-order factors could be accounted for by the second-order factor. Finally, model S 4, in which the second-order factor means were constrained to be identical for boys and girls (i.e., fixed to zero in both groups), did not fit the data 2 significantly worse than model S 3 (χ diff (1)=4.20, p=.04). So we conclude that boys and girls do not Table 6 Fit statistics Belgian sample CFI RMSEA χ 2 diff F 1 Configural invariance F 1a Configural invariance+residuals OA and BD correlated F 1a vs. F 1 : χ 2 diff(2)=48.01, pb.001 F 2 Metric invariance F 2 vs. F 1a : χ 2 diff(11)=4.89, ns F 3 Strong factorial invariance F 3 vs. F 2 :χ 2 diff(9)=112.90, pb.001 F 3a Strong factorial invariance, bar INF, AR and CO F 3a vs. F 2 : χ 2 diff(6)=17.05, ns F 4 Strict factorial invariance F 4 vs. F 3a : χ 2 diff(13)=29.81, pb.01 F 4a Strict factorial invariance, bar INF F 4a vs. F 3a : χ 2 diff(12)=23.31, ns S 1 Introduction 2nd order factor S 1 is identical to F 4a S 2 Metric invariance 2nd order factor S 2 vs. S 1 : χ 2 diff(2)=7.86, ns S 3 Strong factorial invariance 2nd order factor S 3 vs. S 2 : χ 2 diff(2)=4.21, ns S 4 Strict factorial invariance 2nd order factor S 4 vs. S 3 : χ 2 diff(1)b1, ns

Sex differences in latent general and broad cognitive abilities for children and youth: Evidence from higher-order MG-MACS and MIMIC models

Sex differences in latent general and broad cognitive abilities for children and youth: Evidence from higher-order MG-MACS and MIMIC models Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Intelligence 36 (2008) 236 260 Sex differences in latent general and broad cognitive abilities for children and youth: Evidence from higher-order MG-MACS and MIMIC

More information

Conference Presentation

Conference Presentation Conference Presentation Bayesian Structural Equation Modeling of the WISC-IV with a Large Referred US Sample GOLAY, Philippe, et al. Abstract Numerous studies have supported exploratory and confirmatory

More information

Verbal Comprehension. Perceptual Reasoning. Working Memory

Verbal Comprehension. Perceptual Reasoning. Working Memory Abstract The German Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fifth Edition (WISC V; Wechsler, 2017a) includes a five-factor structure (Figure 1), but its Technical Manual (Wechsler, 2017b) CFA analyses

More information

Psychological Assessment

Psychological Assessment Psychological Assessment How Well Is Psychometric g Indexed by Global Composites? Evidence From Three Popular Intelligence Tests Matthew R. Reynolds, Randy G. Floyd, and Christopher R. Niileksela Online

More information

WISC V Construct Validity: Hierarchical EFA with a Large Clinical Sample

WISC V Construct Validity: Hierarchical EFA with a Large Clinical Sample WISC V Construct Validity: Hierarchical EFA with a Large Clinical Sample The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fifth Edition (WISC V; Wechsler, 2014) was published with a theoretical five-factor

More information

Multidimensional Aptitude Battery-II (MAB-II) Clinical Report

Multidimensional Aptitude Battery-II (MAB-II) Clinical Report Multidimensional Aptitude Battery-II (MAB-II) Clinical Report Name: Sam Sample ID Number: 1000 A g e : 14 (Age Group 16-17) G e n d e r : Male Years of Education: 15 Report Date: August 19, 2010 Summary

More information

Chapter 11. Multiple-Sample SEM. Overview. Rationale of multiple-sample SEM. Multiple-sample path analysis. Multiple-sample CFA.

Chapter 11. Multiple-Sample SEM. Overview. Rationale of multiple-sample SEM. Multiple-sample path analysis. Multiple-sample CFA. Chapter 11 Multiple-Sample SEM Facts do not cease to exist because they are ignored. Overview Aldous Huxley Rationale of multiple-sample SEM Multiple-sample path analysis Multiple-sample CFA Extensions

More information

Item response theory analysis of the cognitive ability test in TwinLife

Item response theory analysis of the cognitive ability test in TwinLife TwinLife Working Paper Series No. 02, May 2018 Item response theory analysis of the cognitive ability test in TwinLife by Sarah Carroll 1, 2 & Eric Turkheimer 1 1 Department of Psychology, University of

More information

Stereotype Threat and Group Differences in Test Performance: A Question of Measurement Invariance

Stereotype Threat and Group Differences in Test Performance: A Question of Measurement Invariance Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association 2005, Vol. 89, No. 5, 696 716 0022-3514/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.5.696 Stereotype Threat

More information

Kristin Gustavson * and Ingrid Borren

Kristin Gustavson * and Ingrid Borren Gustavson and Borren BMC Medical Research Methodology 2014, 14:133 RESEARCH ARTICLE Open Access Bias in the study of prediction of change: a Monte Carlo simulation study of the effects of selective attrition

More information

On the Problem of Spurious Non-Linear Effects in Aggregated Scores: Investigating Differentiation of Cognitive Abilities using Item Level Data

On the Problem of Spurious Non-Linear Effects in Aggregated Scores: Investigating Differentiation of Cognitive Abilities using Item Level Data On the Problem of Spurious Non-Linear Effects in Aggregated Scores: Investigating Differentiation of Cognitive Abilities using Item Level Data Dylan Molenaar SEM working group, 15th of March 2018, Amsterdam,

More information

Psychological Science

Psychological Science Psychological Science http://pss.sagepub.com/ On the Nature and Nurture of Intelligence and Specific Cognitive Abilities: The More Heritable, the More Culture Dependent Kees-Jan Kan, Jelte M. Wicherts,

More information

Psychological Science

Psychological Science Psychological Science http://pss.sagepub.com/ On the Nature and Nurture of Intelligence and Specific Cognitive Abilities: The More Heritable, the More Culture Dependent Kees-Jan Kan, Jelte M. Wicherts,

More information

Test and Measurement Chapter 10: The Wechsler Intelligence Scales: WAIS-IV, WISC-IV and WPPSI-III

Test and Measurement Chapter 10: The Wechsler Intelligence Scales: WAIS-IV, WISC-IV and WPPSI-III Test and Measurement Chapter 10: The Wechsler Intelligence Scales: WAIS-IV, WISC-IV and WPPSI-III Throughout his career, Wechsler emphasized that factors other than intellectual ability are involved in

More information

Psychometric Properties of the Norwegian Short Version of the Team Climate Inventory (TCI)

Psychometric Properties of the Norwegian Short Version of the Team Climate Inventory (TCI) 18 Psychometric Properties of the Norwegian Short Version of the Team Climate Inventory (TCI) Sabine Kaiser * Regional Centre for Child and Youth Mental Health and Child Welfare - North (RKBU-North), Faculty

More information

Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research

Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research Timothy A. Brown SERIES EDITOR'S NOTE by David A. Kenny THE GUILFORD PRESS New York London Contents 1 Introduction Uses of Confirmatory Factor Analysis

More information

Statistics & Analysis. Confirmatory Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Modeling of Noncognitive Assessments using PROC CALIS

Statistics & Analysis. Confirmatory Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Modeling of Noncognitive Assessments using PROC CALIS Confirmatory Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Modeling of Noncognitive Assessments using PROC CALIS Steven Holtzman, Educational Testing Service, Princeton, NJ Sailesh Vezzu, Educational Testing

More information

Design of Intelligence Test Short Forms

Design of Intelligence Test Short Forms Empirical Versus Random Item Selection in the Design of Intelligence Test Short Forms The WISC-R Example David S. Goh Central Michigan University This study demonstrated that the design of current intelligence

More information

Equivalence of Q-interactive and Paper Administrations of Cognitive Tasks: Selected NEPSY II and CMS Subtests

Equivalence of Q-interactive and Paper Administrations of Cognitive Tasks: Selected NEPSY II and CMS Subtests Equivalence of Q-interactive and Paper Administrations of Cognitive Tasks: Selected NEPSY II and CMS Subtests Q-interactive Technical Report 4 Mark H. Daniel, PhD Senior Scientist for Research Innovation

More information

Adequacy of Model Fit in Confirmatory Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Models: It Depends on What Software You Use

Adequacy of Model Fit in Confirmatory Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Models: It Depends on What Software You Use Adequacy of Model Fit in Confirmatory Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Models: It Depends on What Software You Use Susan R. Hutchinson University of Northern Colorado Antonio Olmos University of

More information

Common Space Analysis of Several Versions of The Wechsler Intelligence Scale For Children

Common Space Analysis of Several Versions of The Wechsler Intelligence Scale For Children Common Space Analysis of Several Versions of The Wechsler Intelligence Scale For Children Richard C. Bell University of Western Australia A joint analysis was made of three versions of the Wechsler Intelligence

More information

Administration duration for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III and Wechsler Memory Scale-III

Administration duration for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III and Wechsler Memory Scale-III Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 16 (2001) 293±301 Administration duration for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III and Wechsler Memory Scale-III Bradley N. Axelrod* Psychology Section (116B),

More information

Overview of WASI-II (published 2011) Gloria Maccow, Ph.D. Assessment Training Consultant

Overview of WASI-II (published 2011) Gloria Maccow, Ph.D. Assessment Training Consultant Overview of WASI-II (published 2011) Gloria Maccow, Ph.D. Assessment Training Consultant Objectives Describe components of WASI-II. Describe WASI-II subtests. Describe utility of data from WASI- II. 2

More information

The Joint WAIS III and WMS III Factor Structure: Development and Cross-Validation of a Six-Factor Model of Cognitive Functioning

The Joint WAIS III and WMS III Factor Structure: Development and Cross-Validation of a Six-Factor Model of Cognitive Functioning Psychological Assessment Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 2003, Vol. 15, No. 2, 149 162 1040-3590/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/1040-3590.15.2.149 The Joint WAIS III and WMS III

More information

Ante s parents have requested a cognitive and emotional assessment so that Ante can work towards fulfilling his true potential.

Ante s parents have requested a cognitive and emotional assessment so that Ante can work towards fulfilling his true potential. 55 South Street Strathfield 2135 0417 277 124 Name: Ante Orlovic Date Of Birth: 5/6/2001 Date Assessed: 27/5/2013 Reason for Referral: Test Administered: Cognitive Assessment Wechsler Intelligence Scale

More information

Profile Analysis on the WISC-IV and WAIS-III in the low intellectual range: Is it valid and reliable? Simon Whitaker. And.

Profile Analysis on the WISC-IV and WAIS-III in the low intellectual range: Is it valid and reliable? Simon Whitaker. And. Profile Analysis on the WISC-IV and WAIS-III in the low intellectual range: Is it valid and reliable? By Simon Whitaker And Shirley Gordon This paper examines how far it is valid to generate a profile

More information

A MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS TECHNIQUE: STRUCTURAL EQUATION MODELING

A MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS TECHNIQUE: STRUCTURAL EQUATION MODELING A Publication of Vol. Issue 4, September 202, ISSN 2278-4853 ABSTRACT A MULTIVARIATE ANALYSIS TECHNIQUE: STRUCTURAL EQUATION MODELING RINAL B. SHAH* *Assistant Professor, L.J. Institute of Management Studies,

More information

Reliability and interpretation of total scores from multidimensional cognitive measures evaluating the GIK 4-6 using bifactor analysis

Reliability and interpretation of total scores from multidimensional cognitive measures evaluating the GIK 4-6 using bifactor analysis Psychological Test and Assessment Modeling, Volume 60, 2018 (4), 393-401 Reliability and interpretation of total scores from multidimensional cognitive measures evaluating the GIK 4-6 using bifactor analysis

More information

SUBTESTS, FACTORS, AND CONSTRUCTS: WHAT IS BEING MEASURED BY TESTS

SUBTESTS, FACTORS, AND CONSTRUCTS: WHAT IS BEING MEASURED BY TESTS In: Intelligence Quotient ISBN: 978-1-62618-728-3 Editor: Joseph C. Kush 2013 Nova Science Publishers, Inc. Chapter 4 SUBTESTS, FACTORS, AND CONSTRUCTS: WHAT IS BEING MEASURED BY TESTS OF INTELLIGENCE?

More information

Assessing the Fitness of a Measurement Model Using Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA)

Assessing the Fitness of a Measurement Model Using Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) International Journal of Innovation and Applied Studies ISSN 2028-9324 Vol. 17 No. 1 Jul. 2016, pp. 159-168 2016 Innovative Space of Scientific Research Journals http://www.ijias.issr-journals.org/ Assessing

More information

Chapter Standardization and Derivation of Scores

Chapter Standardization and Derivation of Scores 19 3 Chapter Standardization and Derivation of Scores This chapter presents the sampling and standardization procedures used to create the normative scores for the UNIT. The demographic characteristics

More information

Theory and Characteristics

Theory and Characteristics Canadian Journal of School Psychology OnlineFirst, published on September 19, 2008 as doi:10.1177/0829573508324458 Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (2003). RIAS: Reynolds Intellectual Assessment Scales.

More information

Fungible Parameter Estimates in Latent Curve Models

Fungible Parameter Estimates in Latent Curve Models Fungible Parameter Estimates in Latent Curve Models Robert MacCallum The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill Taehun Lee Michael W. Browne UCLA The Ohio State University Current Topics in the Theory

More information

Construct Validity of the WISC-V in Clinical Cases: Exploratory and Confirmatory Factor Analyses of the 10 Primary Subtests

Construct Validity of the WISC-V in Clinical Cases: Exploratory and Confirmatory Factor Analyses of the 10 Primary Subtests 811609ASMXXX10.1177/1073191118811609AssessmentCanivez et al. research-article2018 Article Construct Validity of the WISC-V in Clinical Cases: Exploratory and Confirmatory Factor Analyses of the 10 Primary

More information

Millions of students were administered the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for

Millions of students were administered the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Validity Studies Factor Structure of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children Fourth Edition Among Referred Students Educational and Psychological Measurement Volume 66 Number 6 December 2006 975-983

More information

EFA in a CFA Framework

EFA in a CFA Framework EFA in a CFA Framework 2012 San Diego Stata Conference Phil Ender UCLA Statistical Consulting Group Institute for Digital Research & Education July 26, 2012 Phil Ender EFA in a CFA Framework Disclaimer

More information

Introduction to Survey Data Analysis

Introduction to Survey Data Analysis Introduction to Survey Data Analysis Young Cho at Chicago 1 The Circle of Research Process Theory Evaluation Real World Theory Hypotheses Test Hypotheses Data Collection Sample Operationalization/ Measurement

More information

Understanding the Dimensionality and Reliability of the Cognitive Scales of the UK Clinical Aptitude test (UKCAT): Summary Version of the Report

Understanding the Dimensionality and Reliability of the Cognitive Scales of the UK Clinical Aptitude test (UKCAT): Summary Version of the Report Understanding the Dimensionality and Reliability of the Cognitive Scales of the UK Clinical Aptitude test (UKCAT): Summary Version of the Report Dr Paul A. Tiffin, Reader in Psychometric Epidemiology,

More information

Bifador Modeling in Construct Validation of Multifactored Tests: Implications for Understanding Multidimensional Constructs and Test Interpretation

Bifador Modeling in Construct Validation of Multifactored Tests: Implications for Understanding Multidimensional Constructs and Test Interpretation Canivez, G. L. (2016). Bifactor modeling in construct validation of multifactored tests: Implications for multidimensionality and test interpretation. In K. Schweizer & C. DiStefano (Eds.), Principles

More information

CHAPTER 5 RESULTS AND ANALYSIS

CHAPTER 5 RESULTS AND ANALYSIS CHAPTER 5 RESULTS AND ANALYSIS This chapter exhibits an extensive data analysis and the results of the statistical testing. Data analysis is done using factor analysis, regression analysis, reliability

More information

Chapter 7. Measurement Models and Confirmatory Factor Analysis. Overview

Chapter 7. Measurement Models and Confirmatory Factor Analysis. Overview Chapter 7 Measurement Models and Confirmatory Factor Analysis Some things have to be believed to be seen. Overview Ralph Hodgson Specification of CFA models Identification of CFA models Naming and reification

More information

Introducing the WISC-V Integrated Gloria Maccow, Ph.D., Assessment Training Consultant

Introducing the WISC-V Integrated Gloria Maccow, Ph.D., Assessment Training Consultant Introducing the WISC-V Integrated Gloria Maccow, Ph.D. Assessment Training Consultant Objectives Describe process-oriented assessment. Describe WISC-V Integrated. Illustrate clinical utility of WISC-V

More information

WPPSI -IV Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Fourth Edition Score Report

WPPSI -IV Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Fourth Edition Score Report WPPSI -IV Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Fourth Edition Report Examinee Name Sample Report Date of Report 10/09/2012 Examinee ID 22222 Grade Kindergarten Date of Birth 08/29/2006

More information

Frequently Asked Questions (FAQs)

Frequently Asked Questions (FAQs) I N T E G R A T E D WECHSLER INTELLIGENCE SCALE FOR CHILDREN FIFTH EDITION INTEGRATED Frequently Asked Questions (FAQs) Related sets of FAQs: For general WISC V CDN FAQs, please visit: https://www.pearsonclinical.ca/content/dam/school/global/clinical/canada/programs/wisc5/wisc-v-cdn-faqs.pdf

More information

WPPSI -IV A&NZ Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Fourth Edition: Australian & New Zealand Score Report

WPPSI -IV A&NZ Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Fourth Edition: Australian & New Zealand Score Report WPPSI -IV A&NZ Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Fourth Edition: Australian & New Zealand Report Examinee Name Sample Report Date of Report 03/05/2017 Examinee ID 22222 Year/Grade Foundation

More information

Haplotype Based Association Tests. Biostatistics 666 Lecture 10

Haplotype Based Association Tests. Biostatistics 666 Lecture 10 Haplotype Based Association Tests Biostatistics 666 Lecture 10 Last Lecture Statistical Haplotyping Methods Clark s greedy algorithm The E-M algorithm Stephens et al. coalescent-based algorithm Hypothesis

More information

Introducing WISC-V Spanish Anise Flowers, Ph.D.

Introducing WISC-V Spanish Anise Flowers, Ph.D. Introducing Introducing Assessment Consultant Introducing the WISC V Spanish, a culturally and linguistically valid test of cognitive ability in Spanish for use with Spanish-speaking children ages 6:0

More information

Chapter 3. Basic Statistical Concepts: II. Data Preparation and Screening. Overview. Data preparation. Data screening. Score reliability and validity

Chapter 3. Basic Statistical Concepts: II. Data Preparation and Screening. Overview. Data preparation. Data screening. Score reliability and validity Chapter 3 Basic Statistical Concepts: II. Data Preparation and Screening To repeat what others have said, requires education; to challenge it, requires brains. Overview Mary Pettibone Poole Data preparation

More information

GREEN PRODUCTS PURCHASE BEHAVIOUR- AN IMPACT STUDY

GREEN PRODUCTS PURCHASE BEHAVIOUR- AN IMPACT STUDY ORIGINAL RESEARCH PAPER Commerce GREEN PRODUCTS PURCHASE BEHAVIOUR- AN IMPACT STUDY KEY WORDS: Green Product, Green Awareness, Environment concern and Purchase Decision Sasikala.N Dr. R. Parameswaran*

More information

Mastering Modern Psychological Testing Theory & Methods Cecil R. Reynolds Ronald B. Livingston First Edition

Mastering Modern Psychological Testing Theory & Methods Cecil R. Reynolds Ronald B. Livingston First Edition Mastering Modern Psychological Testing Theory & Methods Cecil R. Reynolds Ronald B. Livingston First Edition Pearson Education Limited Edinburgh Gate Harlow Essex CM20 2JE England and Associated Companies

More information

Comparability of GATE Scores for Immigrants and Majority Group Members: Some Dutch Findings

Comparability of GATE Scores for Immigrants and Majority Group Members: Some Dutch Findings Journal of Applied Psychology 1997, Vol. 82, No. 5, 675-687 Copyright 1997 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0021-9010/97/S3.00 Comparability of GATE Scores for Immigrants and Majority Group

More information

Introduction to Survey Data Analysis. Focus of the Seminar. When analyzing survey data... Young Ik Cho, PhD. Survey Research Laboratory

Introduction to Survey Data Analysis. Focus of the Seminar. When analyzing survey data... Young Ik Cho, PhD. Survey Research Laboratory Introduction to Survey Data Analysis Young Ik Cho, PhD Research Assistant Professor University of Illinois at Chicago Fall 2008 Focus of the Seminar Data Cleaning/Missing Data Sampling Bias Reduction When

More information

Scoring Assistant SAMPLE REPORT

Scoring Assistant SAMPLE REPORT Scoring Assistant SAMPLE REPORT To order, call 1-800-211-8378, or visit our Web site at www.psychcorp.com In Canada, call 1-800-387-7278 In United Kingdom, call +44 (0) 1865 888188 In Australia, call (Toll

More information

CHAPTER 5 DATA ANALYSIS AND RESULTS

CHAPTER 5 DATA ANALYSIS AND RESULTS 5.1 INTRODUCTION CHAPTER 5 DATA ANALYSIS AND RESULTS The purpose of this chapter is to present and discuss the results of data analysis. The study was conducted on 518 information technology professionals

More information

Investigation of the Factor Structure of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Fourth Edition (WAIS IV): Exploratory and Higher Order Factor Analyses

Investigation of the Factor Structure of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Fourth Edition (WAIS IV): Exploratory and Higher Order Factor Analyses Psychological Assessment 2010 American Psychological Association 2010, Vol. 22, No. 4, 827 836 1040-3590/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0020429 Investigation of the Factor Structure of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence

More information

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Group differences in intelligence test performance Wicherts, J.M. Link to publication

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Group differences in intelligence test performance Wicherts, J.M. Link to publication UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Group differences in intelligence test performance Wicherts, J.M. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Wicherts, J. M. (2007). Group differences

More information

1. BE A SQUEAKY WHEEL.

1. BE A SQUEAKY WHEEL. Tips for Parents: Intellectual Assessment of Exceptionally and Profoundly Gifted Children Author: Wasserman, J. D. Source: Davidson Institute for Talent Development 2006 The goal of norm-referenced intelligence

More information

CHAPTER 4 METHOD. procedures. It also describes the development of the questionnaires, the selection of the

CHAPTER 4 METHOD. procedures. It also describes the development of the questionnaires, the selection of the CHAPTER 4 METHOD 4.1 Introduction This chapter discusses the research design, sample, and data collection procedures. It also describes the development of the questionnaires, the selection of the research

More information

The Mahalanobis Distance index of WAIS-R subtest scatter: Psychometric properties in a healthy UK sample

The Mahalanobis Distance index of WAIS-R subtest scatter: Psychometric properties in a healthy UK sample British Journal of Clinical Psychology (1994), 33, 65-69 Printed in Great Britain 6 5 1994 The British Psychological Society The Mahalanobis Distance index of WAIS-R subtest scatter: Psychometric properties

More information

DNA Collection. Data Quality Control. Whole Genome Amplification. Whole Genome Amplification. Measure DNA concentrations. Pros

DNA Collection. Data Quality Control. Whole Genome Amplification. Whole Genome Amplification. Measure DNA concentrations. Pros DNA Collection Data Quality Control Suzanne M. Leal Baylor College of Medicine sleal@bcm.edu Copyrighted S.M. Leal 2016 Blood samples For unlimited supply of DNA Transformed cell lines Buccal Swabs Small

More information

Dealing with Missing Data: Strategies for Beginners to Data Analysis

Dealing with Missing Data: Strategies for Beginners to Data Analysis Dealing with Missing Data: Strategies for Beginners to Data Analysis Rachel Margolis, PhD Assistant Professor, Department of Sociology Center for Population, Aging, and Health University of Western Ontario

More information

Multidimensional Aptitude Battery-II (MAB-II) Extended Report

Multidimensional Aptitude Battery-II (MAB-II) Extended Report Multidimensional Aptitude Battery-II (MAB-II) Extended Report Name: Sam Sample A g e : 30 (Age Group 25-34) Gender: Male Report Date: January 17, 2017 The profile and report below are based upon your responses

More information

Confirmatory factor analysis in Mplus. Day 2

Confirmatory factor analysis in Mplus. Day 2 Confirmatory factor analysis in Mplus Day 2 1 Agenda 1. EFA and CFA common rules and best practice Model identification considerations Choice of rotation Checking the standard errors (ensuring identification)

More information

Glossary of Terms Ability Accommodation Adjusted validity/reliability coefficient Alternate forms Analysis of work Assessment Band Battery

Glossary of Terms Ability Accommodation Adjusted validity/reliability coefficient Alternate forms Analysis of work Assessment Band Battery 1 1 1 0 1 0 1 0 1 Glossary of Terms Ability A defined domain of cognitive, perceptual, psychomotor, or physical functioning. Accommodation A change in the content, format, and/or administration of a selection

More information

FOR TRAINING ONLY! WISC -V Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children -Fifth Edition Score Report

FOR TRAINING ONLY! WISC -V Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children -Fifth Edition Score Report WISC -V Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children -Fifth Edition Report Examinee Name Case RD Sample Date of Report 10/16/2014 Examinee ID 10082014 Grade 5 Date of Birth 04/16/2003 Primary Language English

More information

Research Note. Community/Agency Trust: A Measurement Instrument

Research Note. Community/Agency Trust: A Measurement Instrument Society and Natural Resources, 0:1 6 Copyright # 2013 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 0894-1920 print=1521-0723 online DOI: 10.1080/08941920.2012.742606 Research Note Community/Agency Trust: A Measurement

More information

Chapter 10. Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling. Alexandre J. S. Morin

Chapter 10. Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling. Alexandre J. S. Morin Chapter 10. Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling Alexandre J. S. Morin University of Western Sydney, Australia & University of Sherbrooke, Canada Herbert W. Marsh University of Western Sydney, Australia,

More information

Estimation of multiple and interrelated dependence relationships

Estimation of multiple and interrelated dependence relationships STRUCTURE EQUATION MODELING BASIC ASSUMPTIONS AND CONCEPTS: A NOVICES GUIDE Sunil Kumar 1 and Dr. Gitanjali Upadhaya 2 Research Scholar, Department of HRM & OB, School of Business Management & Studies,

More information

Econ 792. Labor Economics. Lecture 6

Econ 792. Labor Economics. Lecture 6 Econ 792 Labor Economics Lecture 6 1 "Although it is obvious that people acquire useful skills and knowledge, it is not obvious that these skills and knowledge are a form of capital, that this capital

More information

Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the TerraNova-Comprehensive Tests of Basic Skills/5. Joseph Stevens. University of New Mexico.

Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the TerraNova-Comprehensive Tests of Basic Skills/5. Joseph Stevens. University of New Mexico. Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the TerraNova-Comprehensive Tests of Basic Skills/5 Joseph Stevens University of New Mexico Keith Zvoch University of Nevada-Las Vegas Abstract Confirmatory factor analysis

More information

Running head: LURIA MODEL CFA 1

Running head: LURIA MODEL CFA 1 Running head: LURIA MODEL CFA 1 Please use the following citation when referencing this work: McGill, R. J. (in press). Exploring the latent structure of the Luria Model for the KABC-II at school age:

More information

t) I WILEY Gary L. Canivez 1 Stefan C. Dombrowski 2 Marley W. Watkins 3 Abstract RESEARCH ARTICLE

t) I WILEY Gary L. Canivez 1 Stefan C. Dombrowski 2 Marley W. Watkins 3 Abstract RESEARCH ARTICLE Received: 18 September 2017 Revised: 31 January 2018 Accepted: 6 April 2018 DOI: 10.1002/pits.22138 RESEARCH ARTICLE WILEY Factor structure of the WISC-V in four standardization age groups: Exploratory

More information

UK Clinical Aptitude Test (UKCAT) Consortium UKCAT Examination. Executive Summary Testing Interval: 1 July October 2016

UK Clinical Aptitude Test (UKCAT) Consortium UKCAT Examination. Executive Summary Testing Interval: 1 July October 2016 UK Clinical Aptitude Test (UKCAT) Consortium UKCAT Examination Executive Summary Testing Interval: 1 July 2016 4 October 2016 Prepared by: Pearson VUE 6 February 2017 Non-disclosure and Confidentiality

More information

Competence Model of the Teacher in Colleges and Universities ---Based on Research in Hebei Province

Competence Model of the Teacher in Colleges and Universities ---Based on Research in Hebei Province Competence Model of the Teacher in Colleges and Universities ---Based on Research in Hebei Province SHI Xuejun, REN Rongrong Department of Materials Science & Engineering Northeastern University at Qinhuangda

More information

Examining Selection Rates and the Qualifying Standard for the Field Radio Operators Course

Examining Selection Rates and the Qualifying Standard for the Field Radio Operators Course CAB D0012483.A2 / Final September 2005 Examining Selection Rates and the Qualifying Standard for the Field Radio Operators Course Catherine M. Hiatt 4825 Mark Center Drive Alexandria, Virginia 22311-1850

More information

On the Irrifutible Merits of Parceling

On the Irrifutible Merits of Parceling On the Irrifutible Merits of Parceling Todd D. Little Director, Institute for Measurement, Methodology, Analysis and Policy Director & Founder, Stats Camp (Statscamp.org) CARMA Webinar November 10 th,

More information

Factor Analysis of the Korean Adaptation of the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC-K) for Ages 2 ½ through 12 ½ Years

Factor Analysis of the Korean Adaptation of the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC-K) for Ages 2 ½ through 12 ½ Years K-ABC-K Factor Analysis 0 Running Head: K-ABC-K FACTOR ANALYSIS Factor Analysis of the Korean Adaptation of the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC-K) for Ages 2 ½ through 12 ½ Years Note APA

More information

The uses of the WISC-III and the WAIS-III with people with a learning disability: Three concerns

The uses of the WISC-III and the WAIS-III with people with a learning disability: Three concerns The uses of the WISC-III and the WAIS-III with people with a learning disability: Three concerns By Simon Whitaker Published in Clinical Psychology, 50 July 2005, 37-40 Summary From information in the

More information

EFFICACY OF ROBUST REGRESSION APPLIED TO FRACTIONAL FACTORIAL TREATMENT STRUCTURES MICHAEL MCCANTS

EFFICACY OF ROBUST REGRESSION APPLIED TO FRACTIONAL FACTORIAL TREATMENT STRUCTURES MICHAEL MCCANTS EFFICACY OF ROBUST REGRESSION APPLIED TO FRACTIONAL FACTORIAL TREATMENT STRUCTURES by MICHAEL MCCANTS B.A., WINONA STATE UNIVERSITY, 2007 B.S., WINONA STATE UNIVERSITY, 2008 A THESIS submitted in partial

More information

Augmentation: An Implementation Strategy for the No Child Left Behind Act of

Augmentation: An Implementation Strategy for the No Child Left Behind Act of policy report Augmentation: An Implementation Strategy for the No Child Left Behind Act of 2001 Stacy Hicks-Herr Jenny Hoffmann (based on a paper by Margaret Jorgensen, PhD) June 2003 Augmentation: An

More information

Department of Sociology King s University College Sociology 302b: Section 570/571 Research Methodology in Empirical Sociology Winter 2006

Department of Sociology King s University College Sociology 302b: Section 570/571 Research Methodology in Empirical Sociology Winter 2006 Department of Sociology King s University College Sociology 302b: Section 570/571 Research Methodology in Empirical Sociology Winter 2006 Computer assignment #3 DUE Wednesday MARCH 29 th (in class) Regression

More information

Sensitivity Analysis of Nonlinear Mixed-Effects Models for. Longitudinal Data That Are Incomplete

Sensitivity Analysis of Nonlinear Mixed-Effects Models for. Longitudinal Data That Are Incomplete ABSTRACT Sensitivity Analysis of Nonlinear Mixed-Effects Models for Longitudinal Data That Are Incomplete Shelley A. Blozis, University of California, Davis, CA Appropriate applications of methods for

More information

WISC-III profile patterns of learning disabled children

WISC-III profile patterns of learning disabled children University of Nebraska at Omaha DigitalCommons@UNO Student Work 7-1996 WISC-III profile patterns of learning disabled children Russell Goetting University of Nebraska at Omaha Follow this and additional

More information

Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Modeling: Exploratory and Confirmatory Factor Analysis

Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Modeling: Exploratory and Confirmatory Factor Analysis Factor Analysis and Structural Equation Modeling: Exploratory and Confirmatory Factor Analysis Hun Myoung Park International University of Japan 1. Glance at an Example Suppose you have a mental model

More information

RATIONALE for IQ TEST

RATIONALE for IQ TEST W I WECHSLER INTELLIGENCE SCALE for CHILDREN FOURTH EDITION RATIONALE for IQ TEST Identify learning problems Determine potential/performance discrepancy Determine eligibility and need for educational therapy

More information

Semester 2, 2015/2016

Semester 2, 2015/2016 ECN 3202 APPLIED ECONOMETRICS 3. MULTIPLE REGRESSION B Mr. Sydney Armstrong Lecturer 1 The University of Guyana 1 Semester 2, 2015/2016 MODEL SPECIFICATION What happens if we omit a relevant variable?

More information

Introduction to Business Research 3

Introduction to Business Research 3 Synopsis Introduction to Business Research 3 1. Orientation By the time the candidate has completed this module, he or she should understand: what has to be submitted for the viva voce examination; what

More information

Introduction to Survey Data Analysis. Linda K. Owens, PhD. Assistant Director for Sampling & Analysis

Introduction to Survey Data Analysis. Linda K. Owens, PhD. Assistant Director for Sampling & Analysis Introduction to Survey Data Analysis Linda K. Owens, PhD Assistant Director for Sampling & Analysis General information Please hold questions until the end of the presentation Slides available at www.srl.uic.edu/seminars/fall15seminars.htm

More information

1. Understand & evaluate survey. What is survey data? When analyzing survey data... General information. Focus of the webinar

1. Understand & evaluate survey. What is survey data? When analyzing survey data... General information. Focus of the webinar What is survey data? Introduction to Survey Data Analysis Linda K. Owens, PhD Assistant Director for Sampling & Analysis Data gathered from a sample of individuals Sample is random (drawn using probabilistic

More information

Canterbury Christ Church University s repository of research outputs.

Canterbury Christ Church University s repository of research outputs. Canterbury Christ Church University s repository of research outputs http://create.canterbury.ac.uk Please cite this publication as follows: Orsini, A., Pezzuti, L. and Hulbert, S. (2015) The unitary ability

More information

An Empirical Investigation of Consumer Experience on Online Purchase Intention Bing-sheng YAN 1,a, Li-hua LI 2,b and Ke XU 3,c,*

An Empirical Investigation of Consumer Experience on Online Purchase Intention Bing-sheng YAN 1,a, Li-hua LI 2,b and Ke XU 3,c,* 2017 4th International Conference on Economics and Management (ICEM 2017) ISBN: 978-1-60595-467-7 An Empirical Investigation of Consumer Experience on Online Purchase Intention Bing-sheng YAN 1,a, Li-hua

More information

Chapter 5. Data Analysis, Results and Discussion

Chapter 5. Data Analysis, Results and Discussion Chapter 5 Data Analysis, Results and Discussion 5.1 Large-scale Instrument Assessment Methodology Data analysis was carried out in two stages. In the first stage the reliabilities and validities of the

More information

An Application of Categorical Analysis of Variance in Nested Arrangements

An Application of Categorical Analysis of Variance in Nested Arrangements International Journal of Probability and Statistics 2018, 7(3): 67-81 DOI: 10.5923/j.ijps.20180703.02 An Application of Categorical Analysis of Variance in Nested Arrangements Iwundu M. P. *, Anyanwu C.

More information

COSTEFFECTIVENESS: THE FORGOTTEN DIMENSION OF PUBLIC SECTOR PERFORMANCE. Hans de Groot (Innovation and Governance Studies, University of Twente)

COSTEFFECTIVENESS: THE FORGOTTEN DIMENSION OF PUBLIC SECTOR PERFORMANCE. Hans de Groot (Innovation and Governance Studies, University of Twente) COSTEFFECTIVENESS: THE FORGOTTEN DIMENSION OF PUBLIC SECTOR PERFORMANCE Hans de Groot (Innovation and Governance Studies, University of Twente) Bart L. van Hulst (Innovation and Public Sector Efficiency

More information

Hierarchical Factor Structure of the Cognitive Assessment System: Variance Partitions From the Schmid Leiman (1957) Procedure

Hierarchical Factor Structure of the Cognitive Assessment System: Variance Partitions From the Schmid Leiman (1957) Procedure School Psychology Quarterly 2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 26, No. 4, 305 317 1045-3830/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0025973 Hierarchical Factor Structure of the Cognitive Assessment System:

More information

Higher-order models versus direct hierarchical models: g as superordinate or breadth factor?

Higher-order models versus direct hierarchical models: g as superordinate or breadth factor? Psychology Science Quarterly, Volume 50, 2008 (1), p. 21-43 Higher-order models versus direct hierarchical models: GILLES E. GIGNAC 1 Abstract Intelligence research appears to have overwhelmingly endorsed

More information

WPPSI -IV A&NZ Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Fourth Edition: Australian & New Zealand Score Report

WPPSI -IV A&NZ Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Fourth Edition: Australian & New Zealand Score Report WPPSI -IV A&NZ Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Fourth Edition: Australian & New Zealand Report Examinee Name Sample Report Date of Report 28/04/2017 Examinee ID 11111 Year/Grade Date

More information

Bifactor Modeling and the Estimation of Model-Based Reliability in the WAIS-IV

Bifactor Modeling and the Estimation of Model-Based Reliability in the WAIS-IV Multivariate Behavioral Research, 48:639 662, 2013 Copyright Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 0027-3171 print/1532-7906 online DOI: 10.1080/00273171.2013.804398 Bifactor Modeling and the Estimation of

More information

Using the WASI II with the WAIS IV: Substituting WASI II Subtest Scores When Deriving WAIS IV Composite Scores

Using the WASI II with the WAIS IV: Substituting WASI II Subtest Scores When Deriving WAIS IV Composite Scores Introduction Using the WASI II with the WAIS IV: Substituting WASI II Subtest Scores When Deriving WAIS IV Composite Scores Technical Report #2 November 2011 Xiaobin Zhou, PhD Susan Engi Raiford, PhD This

More information

Woodcock Reading Mastery Test Revised (WRM)Academic and Reading Skills

Woodcock Reading Mastery Test Revised (WRM)Academic and Reading Skills Woodcock Reading Mastery Test Revised (WRM)Academic and Reading Skills PaTTANLiteracy Project for Students who are Deaf or Hard of Hearing A Guide for Proper Test Administration Kindergarten, Grades 1,

More information